Références Bibliographiques

  1. Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1994). Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature, 372(6507), 669-72.
  2. Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Fear and the human amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 15(9), 5879-91.
  3. Allport, D. A. (1980). Attention and performances. In G.Claxton (Ed), Cognitive psychology: New direction. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
  4. Allport, D. A. (1989). Visual attention. In M.I. Posner (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive science(pp. 631–682). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  5. Amaral, D. G., Price, J. L., Pitkanen, A., & Carmichael, S. T. (1992). Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In: Aggleton J, (Ed.). The amygdala (pp 1–67). New York: Wiley-Liss.
  6. Baribeau, J. & Roth, R. M.(1996). Performance of subclinical compulsive checkers on putative tests of frontal and temporal lobe memory functions. Journal-of-Nervous-and-Mental-Disease . 184(7), 411-416.
  7. Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994) Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 40, 7–15.
  8. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Anderson, S. W. (1998). Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurosciences 18(1), 428-437.
  9. Bechara, A., Damasio, A.R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S.W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7-15.
  10. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
  11. Blaney, P. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229-246.
  12. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory, American Psychologist, 36, 129-148. 
  13. Bradley, B.P., Mogg, K., & Williams, R. (1995b). Implicit and explicit memory for emotion-congruent information in clinical depression and anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 33, 755-70
  14. Bradley, M. M, Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 25, 49-59
  15. Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K. & Lee, S. (1997). Attentional biases for negative inormation in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. Behavior Research and Therapy, 35, 911-927.
  16. Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J. & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional Bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety. Manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 737-753.
  17. Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Millar, N. Bonham-Carter, C. Fergusson, E. Jenkins, J. & Parr, M. (1997). Attentional biases for emotional faces. Cognition & Emotion, 11, 25-42.
  18. Bradley, B.P. Mogg,-K Millar, N. White, J.(1995a). Selective processing of negative information: Effects of clinical anxiety, concurrent depression, and awareness.
    Journal of Abnormal Psychology . 104(3), 532-536.
  19. Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Millar, N. H. (2000). Covert and overt orienting of attention to emotional faces in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 789-808.
  20. Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., White, J., Groom, C., & de Bono, J. (1999). Attentional bias for emotional faces in generalized anxiety disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology , 38, 267-278.
  21. Breiter, H. C., N. L. Etcoff, P. J. Whalen, W. A. Kennedy, S. L. Rauch, R. L. Buckner, M. M. Strauss, S. E. Human, and B. R. Rosen. (1996). Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron 17, 875-887.
  22. Broadbent, D. E. & Gregory, M. (1967). Perception of emotionally toned words. Nature, 215, 581-584.
  23. Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon Press.
  24. Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and Stress. London: Academic Press.
  25. Broadbent, D. E. (1982). Task combination and selective intake of information. Acta Psychologia, 50, 253-290.
  26. Broks, P., Young, A.W., Marotos, E. J., Coffey, P. J., Calder, A. J., Isaac, C. L., Mayer, A. R., Hodges, J. R., Montaldi, D., Cezayirli, E., Roberts, N., & Halley, D. (1998). Face processing impairment after encephalitis: Amygdala damage and recognition of fear. Neuropsychologia, 36, 59-70.
  27. Burgess, I. S., Jones, L. N., Robertson, S. A., Radcliffe, W. N., Emerson, E., Lawler, P., & Crow, T. J. (1981). The degree of control exerted by phobic and non phobic verbal stimuli over the recognition behaviour of phobic and non phobic subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19, 233-234.
  28. Byrne, A. & Eysenck, M. (1995). Trait anxiety, anxious mood, and threat detection. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 549-562.
  29. Cacioppo, J. T., & Gardner, W. L., (1999). Emotion. Annual Review Psychological, 50, 191, 214.
  30. Calder, A. J., Young, A. W., Rowland, D., Perrett, D. I., Hodges, J. R., & Etcoff, N. L. (1996). Facial emotion recognition after bilateral amygdala damage: Differentially servere impairment of fear. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 699-745.
  31. Camus, J. F. (1996). La psychologie cognitive de l’attention. Paris: Armand Colin/Masson.
  32. Cave, K. R. & Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search. Cognitive psychology, 22, 225-271.
  33. Clark, V. P., Fannon, S., Lai, S., Benson, R., & Bauer, L. (2000). Responses to rare visual target and distractor stimuli using event-related fMRI. Journal of Neurophysiology 83, 3133–3139.
  34. Clark, M. S., Teasdale, J. D., Broadbent, D. E., & Martin, M. (1983). Effect of Mood and Lexical Decision. Bulletin of the psychonomic Society, 21, 175-178.
  35. Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McCelland, J. L. (1990). On the Control of Automatic Processes: A Parallel Distributed Processing Account of the Stroop Effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332-361.
  36. Cohen, J. D., MacWhinney B, Flatt M, Provost J. (1993). PsyScope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 25(2), 257-271.
  37. Compton, R. (2000). Ability to disengage attention predicts negative affect. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 401-415.
  38. Corson, Y. (2002). Variations émotionnelles et mémoire: principaux modèles explicatifs. L’Année psychologique, 102, 109-149.
  39. Dalgleish, T., Mathews, A. & Wood, J. (1999). Inhibitory processes in cognition and emotion: A special case?. In T. DALGLEISH & M. Power (Eds.), The Handbook of Cognition and Emotion. Chichester: Wiley.
  40. Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' Error . G.P. Putnam's Sons, New York.
  41. Davidson, R. J. & Irwin, W. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 11-21. 
  42. Dawkins, K., & Furnham, A. (1989). The colour naming of emotional words. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 383-389.
  43. Derryberry, D. & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-Related Attentional Biases and Their Regulation by Attentional Control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225-236.
  44. De-Ruiter, C. & Brosschot, J. F. (1994). The emotional Stroop interference effect in anxiety: Attentional bias or cognitive avoidance? Behaviour-Research-and-Therapy, 32, 315-319.
  45. Desimone, R. (1999). Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. Humphreys, Glyn W. (Ed); Duncan, John (Ed); et-al. (1999). Attention, space, and action: Studies in cognitive neuroscience. (pp. 13-30). London, Oxford University Press.
  46. Desimone, R., & J. Duncan. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18, 193-222.
  47. Deutsch, P. A. & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70, 80-90.
  48. Dixon, N. F. (1981). Preconscious processing. London : John Wiley.
  49. Dorfman, D. D. (1967). Recognition of taboo words as a function of a priori probability. Journal of personality and social psychology, 7, 1-10.
  50. Dorfman, D. D., Grosberg, J. M., & Kroecher, L. (1965). Recognition of taboo stimuli as a function of exposure time. Journal of personality and social psychology, 2, 552-562.
  51. Driver, J., McLeod, P., & Dienes, Z. (1992). Motion coherence and conjunction search: Implications for guided search theory. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 79-85.
  52. Duncan, J. & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433-458.
  53. Duncan, J. (1980). The locus of interference. Psychological review, 87(3), 272-300.
  54. Duncan, J., Humphreys, G., Ward, R. (1997). Competitive brain activity in visual attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 255-261
  55. Dutta, A., Schweickert, R., Choi, S., & Proctor, R. W. (1995).  Cross-task cross talk in memory and perception. Acta Psychologica, 90, 49-62.
  56. Erikson, C. W. (1963). Perception and personality. In N.J. Wepman and R. W. Heine (eds), Concepts of personality. Chicago: Aldine. 
  57. Eysenck, M. W. (1992). Anxiety: the cognitive perceptive. Hove. UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.
  58. Eysenck, M. W. (1997). Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory. Hove. UK: Psychology Press.
  59. Eysenck, M. W., Mogg, K., May, J., Richards, A., & Mathews, A. (1991). Bias in interpretation of ambiguous sentences related to threat in anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 144-150.
  60. Foa, E. G. & McNally, R. J. (1986). Sensitivity to feared stimuli in obsessive-compulsives: a dichotic listening analysis. Cognitive therapy and research, 10, 477-486.
  61. Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W. & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control setting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030-1044.
  62. Fox, E. (1996). Selective processing of threatening words in anxiety: The role of awareness. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 449-480.
  63. Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681-700.
  64. Fredrickson, B.L.(1998). The role of positive Emotions in positive psychology. American Psychologist, 56(3) 218-226.
  65. Friedrich, F. J., Henik, A., & Tzelgov, J. (1991). Automatic processes in lexical access and spreading activation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 17, (3), 792-806.
  66. Gainotti, G., (1989) The meaning of emotional disturbances resulting from unilateral brain injury. In G. Gainotti and C. Caltagirone (Eds.), Emotions and the Brain. Berlin: Spingler.
  67. Gerrig, R. J., & Bower, G. H. (1982). Emotional influences on word recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 19, 197-200.
  68. Gilligan, S. G., & Bower, G. H. (1984). Cognitive consequences of emotional arousal. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan & R. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognitions, and behavior. (pp.547-588). New York: Cambridge Press.
  69. Glaser, M. O., & Glaser, W. R. (1982). Time course analysis of the Stroop phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 875-894.
  70. Gotlib, I. H., & Cane, O. B. (1987). Construct accessibility and clinical depression: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 199-204.
  71. Gotlib, I. H., & McCann, C. D. (1984). Construct accessibility and depression: An examination of cognitive and affective factors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 427-439.
  72. Graham, F. K. (1992). Attention: The heartbeat, the blink, and the brain.(In B. A. Campbell, H. Hayne, & R. Richardson (Eds.), Attention and information processing in infants and adults (pp. 3—29). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  73. Gray, J. A. (1982). The neurophsychology of anxiety. Oxford University Press, New York.
  74. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 87-108.
  75. Greenwald, A.G., & Rosenberg, K.E. (1978). Sequential effects of distracting stimuli in a selective attention reaction time task. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance, VII. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 487-504.
  76. Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Regional brain electrical asymmetries discriminate between depressed and nondepressed subjects during cognitive task performance. Biological Psychiatry . 42 , 1039-1050.
  77. Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen D., & Eelen P., (1999). Eye Movement Registration as a Continuous Index of Attention Deployment: Date from a Group of Spider Anxious Students. Cognition and Emotion, 13 (4), 419-434.
  78. Hill, A. B., & Dutton, F. (1989). Notes and shorter communications: Depression and selective attention to self-esteem threatening words. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 915-917.
  79. Hirst, W. (1986). The psychology of attention. In J. E. LeDoux and W. Hirst (Eds.), Mind and brain: Dialogues in cognitive neuroscience. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  80. Hirst, W., Spelke, E. S., Reaves, C. C., Caharack, G., & Neisser, U. (1980). Dividing attention without alternation or automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 98-117.
  81. Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1-66.
  82. Hout, M. A. van den, Jong, P. de & Kindt, M. (2000). Masked fear words produce increased SCRs: An anomaly for Öhman’s theory of pre-attentive processing in anxiety. Psychophysiology, 37, 383-388.
  83. Izard, C. E. (1993). Four systems of emotion activation: Cognitive and noncognitive processes. Psychological Review, 100 , 68-90.
  84. Johnston, W.A. & Dark, V.J. (1986). Selective attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 37, 43-75.
  85. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall.
  86. Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of Attention, Chap. 2 (pp. 29-61). Orlando: Academic Press.
  87. Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., Davidson, R. J., & Kelley, A. E. (2001). The primate amygdala mediates acute fear but not the behavioral and physiological components of anxious temperament. Journal of Neuroscience. 21, 2067-2074.
  88. Karno, M., Golding, J. M., Sorenson, S. B., Burnam, M. A. (1988). The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in five US communities.  Arch Gen Psychiatry.  45(12),1094-9.
  89. Kirino, E., Belger, A., Goldman-Rakic, P., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Prefrontal activation evoked by infrequent target and novel stimuli in a visual target detection task: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience 20, 6612– 6618.
  90. Kitayama, S. (1990). Interaction between affect and cognition in word perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 209-217.
  91. Kitayama, S. (1991). Impairment of perception by positive and negative affect. Cognition-and-Emotion, 5(4), 255-274.
  92. Klauer, K. C. & Musch, J. (in press). Affective priming: Findings and theories. In K.C. Klauer & J. Musch (Eds.), The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion . Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  93. Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color naming. American Journal of Psychology, 77, 576-588.
  94. Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557-558.
  95. LaBerge, D. (1981). Automatic information processing: A review. En J. Long y A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and Performance IX (pp. 173-186). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
  96. LaBerge, D. (1995). Attentional processing: The brain's art of mindfulness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  97. Lane R. D. & Nadal, L. (2000). Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. New York, US: Oxford University Press.
  98. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1998). Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biological Psychiatry, 44 ,1248-1263
  99. Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American Psychologist, 50, 372-385.
  100. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. M. (1999). Affect and the startle reflex. In: Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., et al. (Eds.). Startle modification: Implications for neuroscience, cognitive science, and clinical science. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.
  101. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1995). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings. NIMH Cent. Stud. Emot. Atten., Univ. FL.
  102. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, M. M. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation and action. In: P.J. Lang, R.F. Simons, M.T. Balaban, editors. Attention and Orienting: Sensory and Motivational Processes. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  103. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaption. New York: Ronald Press.
  104. LeDoux, J. E. (1986). Sensory systems and emotion: A model of affective processing. Integrative Psychiatry, 4, 237-248.
  105. LeDoux, J. E. (1989). Cognitive-emotional Interactions in the brain. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 267-289.
  106. LeDoux, J. E. (1992). Emotion and the amygdala. In: The Amygdala, (pp. 339-351) ed. J. P. Aggleton. Wiley-Liss.
  107. LeDoux, J. E. (1994a). Memory versus emotional memory in the brain. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.). The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. New York: Oxford U. Press.
  108. LeDoux, J. E. (1994b). Emotion, memory and the brain. Scientific American, 270, 32-39.
  109. LeDoux, J. E. (1995). In search of an emotional system in the brain: Leaping from fear to emotion and consciousness, in: M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences, (pp. 1049–1061). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
  110. LeDoux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain. New York : Simon and Schuster.
  111. LeDoux, J. E. (2000), Emotion circuits in the brain, Annual Reviews in the Neurosciences, 23, 155-184. Cover pages 173-177.
  112. Leventhal, H. (1979). A perceptual motor processing model of emotion. In P. Pliner, K. Blankenstein, & I. M. Spiegel (Eds.), Perception of emotion in self and others: Vol. 5, New York: Plenum Press.
  113. Leventhal, H. (1984). A perceptual motor theory of emotion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology: Vol. 17. New York: Academic Press.
  114. Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. (1987). The relationship of emotion to cognition: A functional approach to a semantic controversy, Cognition and Emotion, 1, 3-28
  115. Levy, L. H. (1958). Perceptual defense in tactual perception. Journal of personality, 26m 467-468.
  116. Lewis, J. L. (1970). Semantic processing of unattended massages during dichotic listening. Journal of experimental psychology, 85, 225-228.
  117. Logan, G.. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task. Memory and Cognition, 7, 166-174.
  118. Lundqvist, K., Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (1999). The Face of Wrath: Critical Features for Conveying Facial Threat. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 691-711.
  119. MacLean, P. D. (1949). Psychosomatic disease and the "visceral brain": recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosomatic Medicine, 11, 338-353.
  120. MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163-203.
  121. MacLeod, C., & Hagan, R. (1992). Individual differences in the selective processing of threatening information, and emotional responses to a stressful life event. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 151-161.
  122. MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. M. (1991a). Cognitive-experimental approaches to the emotional disorders.. In Martin, Paul R. (Ed); et-al. (1991). Handbook of behavior therapy and psychological science: An integrative approach.Pergamon general psychology series, Vol. 164. (pp. 116-150). New York, NY, USA: Pergamon Press, Inc.
  123. MacLeod, C. & Mathews, A. (1991b). Biased cognitive operations in anxiety. Accessibility of information or assignment of processing priorities? Behavior Research and Therapy, 29, 599-610.
  124. MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15 - 20.
  125. MacLeod, C. M., & Rutherford, E. M. (1992). Anxiety and the selective processing of emotional information: Mediating roles of awareness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimulus materials. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 497-491.
  126. MacLeod, C., Tata, P., & Mathews, A. M. (1987). Perception of emotionally valenced information in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26,(1), 67-68.
  127. Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and Body: Psychology of Emotion and Stress . New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company, 1984.
  128. Martin, M., Williams, R. M., & Clark, D. M. (1991). Does anxiety lead to selective processing of threat-related information? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 147-160.
  129. Mathews, A. (1988). Anxiety and the processing of threatening information. In V. Hamilton, G. H. Bower & N. H. Frijda (Eds.), Cognitive Perspectives on Emotion and Motivation (pp. 265-284). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.
  130. Mathews, A. (1990). Why sorry ? The cognitive function of anxiety. Behaviour Research and therapy, 28, 455-468.
  131. Mathews, A. (1993). Biases in processing emotional information. The Psychologist, 6, 493-499.
  132. Mathews, A. & MacKintosh, B. (1998). Cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22 , 539-560.
  133. Mathews, A., MacKintosh, B. & Fulcher, E. P. (1997). Cognitive biases in anxiety and attention to threat.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 340-345.
  134. Mathews, A. M. & Mackintosh, B, (1998). A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 22, 539-560.
  135. Mathews, A. M. & Milroy, R. (1994). Processing of emotional meaning in anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 8, 535-554.
  136. Mathews, A. & MacLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 23, 563-569.
  137. Mathews, A. & MacLeod, C. (1986). Discrimination of threat cues without awareness in anxiety states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 131-138.
  138. Mathews, A. & MacLeod, C. (1987). An information processing approach to anxiety. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1, 24-30.
  139. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1988). Anxiety and the allocation of attention to threat. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40 , 653-670.
  140. Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1994). Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 95, 131-138.
  141. Mathews, A. & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition and Emotion 16, 331-354.
  142. Mathews, A., Mogg, K., Kentish, J., & Eysenck, M. (1995). Effect of psychological treatment on cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 33, 293-303.
  143. Mathews, A., Mogg, K., May, J., & Eysenck, M. (1990). Implicit and explicit memory bias in anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 236-240.
  144. Mathews, A. M., & Sebastian, S. (1993). Suppression of emotional Stroop effects by fear-arousal. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 517–530.
  145. Matthews, G., & Wells, A. (1999). The cognitive science of attention and emotion. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 171-192). New York: Wiley.
  146. Mattia, J. I., Heimberg, R. G., & Hope, D. A. (1993). The revised Stroop colour naming task in social phobics: diagnostic and treatments outcome implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 305-313.
  147. McCarthy, G., Luby, M., Gore, J., & Goldman-Rakic, P. (1997). Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77,1630 –1634.
  148. McKenna, F. & Sharma, D. (1995). Intrusive cognitions: an investigation of the emotional Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21(6), 1595-1607
  149. McKenna, F. & Sharma, D. (1995). Intrusive cognitions: an investigation of the emotional Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21(6), 1595-1607.
  150. McLeod, M. C. (1977). A dual-task response modality effect: Support for multiprocessor models of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 651-667.
  151. McNally, R. J., Amir, N., Louro, C. E., Lukach, B. M., Riemann, B. C.& Calamari, J. E. (1994). Cognitive processing of idiofraphic e;oional information in panic disorder. Behaviour research and therapy, 32, 119-122.
  152. Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1994). EPIC computational models of psychological refractory-period effects in human multiple-task performance. (EPIC Tech. Rep. No. 2, TR-94/ONR-EPIC-2). Ann Arbor, University of Michigan, Department of Psychology.
  153. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167-202.
  154. Minard, J. G. (1965). Response-bias and the emotional disorders. Psychological science. 3, 65-69.
  155. Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 36, 809-848.
  156. Mogg, K. & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Orienting of attention to threatening facial expression presented under conditions of restricted awareness. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 713-740.
  157. Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Hallowell, N. (1994). Attentional bias to threat: Roles of trait anxiety, stressful events, and awareness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 47, 841-864.
  158. Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Millar, N., & White, J. (1995). Cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder: A follow-up study. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 33, 927-935.
  159. Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Williams, R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: The role of awareness. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34 (1), 17-36.
  160. Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304-311.
  161. Mogg, K., Kentish, J. & Bradley, B. P. (1993). Effects of anxiety and awareness on colour-identification latencies for emotional words. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 559-567.
  162. Mogg, K., Mathews, A., Bird, C., & Macgregor-Morris, R. (1990). Effects of stress and anxiety on the processing of threat stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1230-1237.
  163. Mogg, K., Mathews, A., & Eysenck, M.W. (1992). Attentional bias to threat in clinical anxiety states. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 149-159.
  164. Mogg, K., Mathews, A., Eysenck, M. & May, J. (1991). Biased cognitive operations in anxiety: artefact, processing priorities, or attentional search? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 459-467.
  165. Mogg, K., Mathews, A. & Weinman, J. (1987). Memory bias in clinical anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 94-98.
  166. Mogg, K., Mathews, A., & Weinman, J. (1989). Selecting processing of threat cues in anxiety states: A replication. Behavior Research and Therapy, 27, 317-323.
  167. Mogg, K., Millar, N. & Bradley, B. P. (2000). Biases in eye movements to threatening facial expressions in generalised anxiety disorder and depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 695-704.
  168. Morgan, M. A., Romanski, L. M., and LeDoux, J. E. (1993). Extinction of emotional learning: contribution of medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience Letters 163, 109-113.
  169. Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Yound, A. W., Calder, A. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1996). A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, 812-815.
  170. Navon, D. and Gopher, D. (1979), On the economy of the human processing system, Psychological Review 86, 214--253.
  171. Navon, D. & Margalit, B. (1983). Allocation of attention according to informativeness in visual recognition. Quarterly Journal of experimental psychology, 35a, 497-512.
  172. Newell, A. Rosenbloom, P. S., Laird, J. E. (1989) Symbolic Architectures for Cognition. In M. Posner (ed.) Foundations of Cognitive Science, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
  173. Niedenthal, P. M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Setterlund, M. B. (1997). Being happy and seeing “happy”: Emotional state mediates visual word recognition. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 403–432
  174. Niedenthal, P. M., & Kitayama, S. (1994). The Heart’s Eye : Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention. New York : Academic Press.
  175. Niedenthal, P. M., & Setterlund, M. B. (1994). Emotion congruence in perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 401-411.
  176. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shaprio (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation. (Vol. 4, pp. 1-18). New York: Plenum.
  177. Oatley, K. (1987). Cognitive science and the understanding of emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 3(1), 209-216.
  178. Oatley, K. (1999). Emotion. Wilson, R. A. & Keil, F. C. (Eds.), The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences. London, England.
  179. Oatley, K. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1985). Sketch for a cognitive theory of emotions, Cognitive Science Research Papers CSRP 045, University of Sussex.
  180. Oatley, K. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 29-50.
  181. Öhman, A. (1979). The orienting response, attention, and learning: An information processing perspective. In H. D. Kimmel, E. H. van Olst, & J. F. Orlebeke (Eds.), The orienting reflex in humans (pp. 443-471). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  182. Öhman, A. (1992). Orienting and attention: Preferred preattentive processing of potentially phobic stimuli. In B. A. Campbell, R. Richardson, & H. Haynes (Eds.) Attention and information processing in infants and adults: Perspectives from human and animal research (pp. 263-295). Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.
  183. Öhman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives, and information processing mechanisms. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 511-536). New York: Guilford Press.
  184. Öhman, A., & Ursin, H. (1979). On the sufficiency of a Pavlovian conditioning model for coping with the complexities of neurosis. The Behavioral and Brain Science, 2, 179-180.
  185. Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466-478.
  186. Öhman, A., Lundqvist, D. & Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: A threat advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 381-396.
  187. Öhman, A. & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483-522.
  188. Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1994). "Unconscious anxiety": Phobic responses to masked stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 231-240.
  189. Öhman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1998). Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: Expectancies for aversive outcomes following non-recognized fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 69-82.
  190. Olafson, K. M. & Ferraro, R. (2001). Effects of emotional state on lexical decision performance. Brain and Cognition, 45, 15-20.
  191. Papez, J. W. (1937). A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 38: 725-744
  192. Parasuraman, R. (1998). The attentive brain. Cambridge, MIT Press.
  193. Parkinson, L & Rachman, S (1981b). Intrusive thoughts: the effects of an uncontrived stress. Advances in Behaviour Research & Therapy, 3, 111-118.
  194. Philippot, P. (1993). Inducing and assessing differentiated emotional feeling states in the laboratory. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 171-193.
  195. Posner, M. I. (1995). Attention in cognitive neurosciences: An overview. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge, Mass: MIT.
  196. Posner, M. I., & DiGirolamo G. (1998). Executive attention: conflict, target detection and cognitive control. In Parasuraman R, (Eds). The attentive brain (pp. 401–23). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  197. Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 109, 160-174.
  198. Postman, L. (1953). On the problem of perceptual defence. Psychological Review, 60, 298-306.
  199. Powell, M. & Hemsley, D. R. (1984). Depression: a breakdown of perceptual defence? British Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 358-362.
  200. Power, M.J. and Dalgleish, T. (1997). Cognition and Emotion: From Order to Disorder. Hove: Psychology Press.
  201. Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380-391.
  202. Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with outliers. Psychological Review, 114 , 510-532.
  203. Richards, A. & Millwood, B. (1989). Colour-identification of differentially valenced words in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion , 3, 171-176.
  204. Riemann, B. C. & McNally, R. J. (1995). Cognitive processing of personally relevant information. Cognition and Emotion., 9, 325-340
  205. Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and Emotion. Oxford University Press, 1999.
  206. Rolls. E. T., Hornak, J., Wade, D., & McGrath, J. (1994). Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 1518-1524.
  207. Sander, D. & Koenig, O. (2001).Vers un modèle computationnel des mécanismes émotionnels. In J. Caron-Pargue, V. Nyckees, & H. Paugam-Moisy (Eds.). La cognition entre individu et société : des modèles et des méthodes. Hermès.
  208. Schneider, W. & Fisk, A. (1983). Attention Theory and Mechanisms for SkilledPerformance, in Magill, R., (Ed.), Memory and The Control of Action (pp.119-143). North-Holland Publishing, Amsterdam.
  209. Schotte, D. E., McNally, R. J., & Turner, M. L. (1990). A dichotic listening analysis of body weight concern in bulimia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 109 –113
  210. Small, S. A. (1985). The effet of mood on wood recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 453-455.
  211. Small, S. A. & Robins, C. R. (1988). The influence of induced depressed mood on visual recognition thresholds: predictive ambiguity of associative network models of mood and cognition. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 12, 295-304.
  212. Speisman, J. C., Lazarus, R. S., Mordkoff, A., & Davison, L. (1964). Experimental reduction of stress based on ego-defense theory. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 367-380.
  213. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.  Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.
  214. Suzuki et Tokita (1999) Visual Evoked Potentials Elicited by Schematic Faces. 4 回日本顔学会大会フォラム顔学 '99 予稿集 , 4, 52.
  215. Taylor, D. A. (1977). Time course of context effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 404-426.
  216. Teasdale, J. D. & Barnard, P. J. (1993). Affect, Cognition and Change: Re-modelling Depressive Thought. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  217. Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571-590.
  218. Trandel, D. V. & McNally, R. J. (1987). Perception of threat cues in post-traumatic stress disorder: semantic processing without awareness? Behavior Research and Therapy, 29, 451-457.
  219. Treisman, A. (1960). Contextual cues in dichotic listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242-248.
  220. Treisman, A. & Gormican, S. (1988) Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95(1),15-48.
  221. Treisman, A., & Souther, J. (1985). Search asymmetry: A diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 285-310.
  222. Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading,MA: Addison-Westley.
  223. Underwood, G. (1976). Semantic interference from unattended printed words. British Journal of Psychology, 67,327-338.
  224. Underwood, G. (1977). Facilitation from attended and unattended messages. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 99-106.
  225. Versace, R. & Padovan, C. (2001). Affect, émotion et mémoire à long terme : un aperçu des travaux en psychologie cognitive. Canal Psy. 47 , 8-10.
  226. Watts, F. N., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., & Trezise, L. (1986). Colour naming of phobia-related words. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 97-108.
  227. Wells, A. & Matthews, G. (1994). Attention and Emotion. Hove, UK : Psychology Press.
  228. White, M. (1996). Automatic Affective Appraisal of Words. Cognition and Emotion, 12(2), 199-212.
  229. Wichens, C. D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In R. Nicherson (Ed.), Attention and Performances VIII. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
  230. Wichens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Pqrasuraman and D. R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of Attention. Orlando, FL.: Academic.
  231. Williams, J. M. G. & Broadbent, K. (1986). Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 144-149.
  232. Williams J. M. G. (1992). The Psychological Treatment of Depression: A guide to theory and practice of cognitive behaviour therapy. Routledge: London.
  233. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews. A., MacLeod, C. (1996). The Emotional Stroop Task and Psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24.
  234. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A.(1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Chichester: Wiley.
  235. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. (1997) Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). Chichester: UK: Wiley.
  236. Yantis, S. (1998). Control of visual attention.In H. Pashler (Ed.), Attention (pp. 223-256). Hove, England: Psychology Press.
  237. Yantis, S. & Johnston, J. C. (1990). On the locus of visual selection: Evidence from focused attention tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 135-149.
  238. Yiend, J. & Mathews. A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(3), 665-681.
  239. Young, A. W., Aggleton, J. P., Hellawell, D. J., Johnson, M., Broks, P., & Hanley, J. R. (1995). Face processing impairments after amygdalotomy. Brain. 118, 15–24.
  240. Yule, W. (1992). Post-traumatic stress disorder in child survivors of shipping disasters: The sinking of the "Jupiter." Psychotherapy-and-Psychosomatics. 57(4): 200-205.
  241. Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking. Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist , 35 , 151–175.
  242. Zajonc, R. B., Murphy, S. T., & McIntosh, D. N. (1993). Brain temperature and subjective emotional experience. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (pp. 209-220). New York: Guilford.
  243. Zink, C. F., Pagnoni, G, Martin, M. E, Dhamala, M, Berns, G. S.(2003). Human striatal response to salient nonrewarding stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience. 23, 8092-8097.