Bibliographie

  1. Alderete, J. (1995). Faithfulness to Prosodic Heads. Communication présentée à The Derivational Residue in Phonology Conference, Tilburg University.
  2. Allen, George D. et Hawkins, S. (1978). The Development of Phonological Rhythm. Dans A. Bell & Joan B. Hooper (Eds.), Syllables and Segments (pp. 173-185). Amsterdam: North Holland.
  3. Allen, George D. et Hawkins, S. (1980). Phonological Rhythm: Definition and Development. Child Phonology, 1, 227-256.
  4. Anderson, J. L., Morgan, J. L. et White, K. S. (2003). A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech, 46(2-3), 155-182.
  5. Angoujard, J. P. (1997). Théorie de la syllabe : rythme et qualité. Paris : CNRS Editions.
  6. Angoujard, J. P. (2006). Phonologie déclarative. Paris: CNRS Editions.
  7. Anttila, A. (1997). Deriving Variation from Grammar. Dans Variation, Change, and Phonological Theory (pp. 35-68). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
  8. Anttila, A. et Cho, Y.-M. Y. (1998). Variation and change in Optimality Theory. Lingua, 104(1-2), 31-56.
  9. Archibald, J. (1996). Second Language Acquisition. Dans W. O’Grady & M. Dobrovolsky (Eds.), Contemporary Linguistic Analysis, An Introduction (pp. 471-504). Toronto: Copp Clark Ltd.
  10. Bakovic, E. (2000). Harmony, Dominance and Control. Ph.D. Thesis, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ.
  11. Barlow, J. A. et Gierut, J. A. (1999). Optimality Theory in phonological acquisition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(6), 1482-1498.
  12. Bernhardt, B. H. et Stemberger, J. P. (1998). Handbook of phonological development from the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. San Diego: Academic Press.
  13. Bertoncini, J., Floccia, C., Mehler, J. et Nazzi, T. (1995). Morae and syllables: rhythmical basis of speech representations in neonates. Language and Speech, 38(4), 311-329.
  14. Bijeljac-Babic, R., Bertoncini, J. et Mehler, J. (1993). How Do 4-Day-Old Infants Categorize Multisyllabic Utterances? Developmental psychology, 29(4), 711-721.
  15. Blevins, J. (1995). The Syllable in Phonological Theory. Dans J. A. Goldsmith (Ed.), The Handbook of Phonological Theory (pp. 206-244). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
  16. Blevins, J. (2004). Evolutionary phonology: the emergence of sound patterns. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  17. Boltanski, J.-E. (1999). Nouvelles directions en phonologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
  18. Braud, V. (2003). Acquisition de la prosodie chez des enfants francophones : les phénomènes de troncations. Thèse de doctorat : Sciences du langage, Université de Nantes.
  19. Casagrande, J. (1984). The Sound System of French. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
  20. Charette, M. (1991). Conditions on Phonological Government. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  21. Chomsky, N. et Halle, M. (1968). The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.
  22. Clements, G. N. (1976). Palatalization: Linking or Assimilation? Communication présentée aux Papers from the Chicago Linguistic Society, Chicago.
  23. Clements, G. N. (1985). The Geometry of Phonological Features. Phonology, 2, 225-252.
  24. Clements, G. N. (1990). The Role of the Sonority Cycle in Core Syllabification. Dans John C. Kingston & Mary E. Beckman (Eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology 1: Between the Grammar and Physics of Speech (pp. 283-333). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  25. Clements, G. N. et Hume, Elizabeth V. (1995). The Internal Organization of Speech Sounds. Dans J. A. Goldsmith (Ed.), The Handbook of Phonological Theory (pp. 245-306). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
  26. Clements, G. N. et Keyser, Samuel J. (1983). CV Phonology: A Generative Theory of the Syllable. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  27. Cole, J. et Kisseberth, C. (1994). An Optimal Domains Theory of Harmony. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences, 24(2), 101-114.
  28. Crelin, Edmund S. (1987). The Human Vocal Tract: Anatomy, Function, Development, and Evolution. New York: Vantage Press.
  29. Davis, S. et Hammond, M. (1995). On the Status of Onglides in American English. Phonology, 12(2), 159-182.
  30. Davis, B. L. et MacNeilage, P. F. (1995). The articulatory basis of babbling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(6), 1199-1211.
  31. Dell, F. (1984). L’accentuation des phrases en français. Dans F. Dell, D. Hirst & J.-R. Vergnaud (Eds.), Forme sonore du langage (pp. 65-122). Paris: Hermann.
  32. Demuth, K. (1995). Markedness and the Development of Prosodic Structure. Communication présentée à Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society (NELS), Amherst.
  33. Demuth, K. (1996). The Prosodic Structure of Early Words. Dans J. L. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), From Signal to Syntax: Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition (pp. 171-184). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  34. Demuth, K. et Fee, E. J. (1995). Minimal Words in Early Phonological Development. Ms.
  35. Demuth, K. et Johnson, M. (2003). Truncation to subminimal words in early French. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique, 48(3/4), 211-241
  36. Deville, G. (1891). Notes sur le développement du langage II. Revue de linguistique et de philologie comparée, 24, 10-42, 128-146, 242-257, 300-320.
  37. Dinnsen, D. A. (1996). Context-sensitive underspecification and the acquisition of phonemic contrasts. Journal of Child Language, 23(1), 57-79.
  38. Durand, J. et Lyche, C. (2001). Des règles aux contraintes en phonologie générative. Revue Québécoise de Linguistique, 30(1), 91-154.
  39. Edwards, M. L. (1996). Word position effects in the production of fricatives. Dans Proceedings of the UBC International Conference on Phonological Acquisition. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
  40. Enstrom, D. H. (1982). Infant Labial, Apical and Velar Stop Productions: A Voice Onset Time Analysis. Phonetica Basel, 39(1), 47-60.
  41. Ewen, C. J. (1982). The Internal Structure of Complex Segments. Dans The Structure of Phonological Representation (pp. 27-67). Dordrecht: Foris.
  42. Eychenne, J. (2006). Aspects de la phonologie du schwa dans le français contemporain. Optimalité, visibilité prosodique, gradience. Thèse de doctorat : Sciences du langage, Université de Toulouse 2.
  43. Farwell, C. B. (1976). Some Strategies in the Early Production of Fricatives. Papers and reports on child language development, 12, 97-104.
  44. Fikkert, P. (1994). On the Acquisition of Prosodic Structure. Dordrecht: ICG Printing.
  45. Fikkert, P. et Levelt, C. (à paraître). How does place fall into place? The lexicon and emergent constraints in the developing phonological grammar. Dans P. Avery, B. E. Dresher & K. Rice (Eds.), Contrast in phonology: Perception and Acquisition. Berlin: Mouton.
  46. Fudge, E. C. (1969). Syllables. Journal of Linguistics, 5, 253-286.
  47. Gerard, D. (1891). Notes sur le développement du langage II. Revue de linguistique et de philologie comparée, 24, 10-42, 128-146, 242-257, 300-320.
  48. Gernsbacher, M. A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 113(2), 256-281.
  49. Givón, T. (1995). Markedness as meta-iconicity: Distributional and cognitive correlates of syntactic structure. Dans Functionalism and Grammar (pp. 25-70). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  50. Gnanadesikan, Amalia E. (1995/2004). Markedness and Faithfulness Constraints in Child Phonology. Dans R. Kager, J. Pater & W. Zonneveld (Eds.), Constraints in Phonological Acquisition (pp. 73-108). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  51. Goad, H. (1996). Consonant Harmony in Child Language: Evidence Against Coronal Underspecification. Communication présentée à Proceedings of the UBC International Conference on Phonological Acquisition, Somerville, MA.
  52. Goad, H. (2000). Assimilation phenomena and initial constraint ranking in early grammars. Communication présentée à 24th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston University.
  53. Goad, H. (2006). Are children’s grammars rogue grammars? Glide substitution in branching onsets. Recherches linguistiques de Vincennes, 35, 103-132.
  54. Goad, H. et Brannen, K. (2000). Syllabification at the Right Edge of Words: Parallels Between Child and Adult Grammars. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics, 15(1), 1-16.
  55. Goad, H. et Rose, Y. (2004). Input Elaboration, Head Faithfulness and Evidence for Representation in the Acquisition of Left-edge Clusters in West Germanic. Dans R. Kager, J. Pater & W. Zonneveld (Eds.), Constraints in Phonological Acquisition (pp. 109-157). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  56. Goldsmith, J. (1976). An Overview of Autosegmental Phonology. Linguistic Analysis, 2, 23-68.
  57. Greenberg, Joseph.H (Ed.). (1966). Universals of language (2 ed.): The M.I.T Press.
  58. Greenberg, Joseph.H (Ed.). (1978). Universals of human language. Vol 2: Phonology (Vol. 2): Stanford University Press.
  59. Gussenhoven, C. et Jacobs, H. (2005). Understanding phonology (2nd ed.). London: Hoddor Arnold.
  60. Hale, M. et Reiss, C. (1998). Formal and Empirical Arguments Concerning Phonological Acquisition. Linguistic Inquiry, 29(4), 656-683.
  61. Halle, M. (1992). Phonological Features. Dans W. Bright (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of Linguistics (Vol. 3, pp. 207-212). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  62. Halle, M. et Vergnaud, J.-R. (1978). Metrical Structures in Phonology. Ms.
  63. Halle, M. et Vergnaud, J.-R. (1987). An Essay on Stress. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  64. Hammond, M., Moravcsik, E. et Wirth, J. (1988). Language typology and linguistic explanation. Dans M. Hammond, E. Moravcsik & J. Wirth (Eds.), Studies in Syntactic Typology (pp. 1-24). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
  65. Harris, J. (1990). Segmental Complexity and Phonological Government. Phonology, 7(2), 255-300.
  66. Harris, J. (1994). English Sound Structure. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
  67. Harris, J. (1997). Licensing Inheritance: An Integrated Theory of Neutralisation. Phonology, 14(3), 315-370.
  68. Hayes, B. (1985). Iambic and Trochaic Rhythm in Stress Rules. Communication présentée à Proceedings of the 11th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley, CA.
  69. Hayes, B. (1995). Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  70. Hayes, B. et Steriade, D. (2004). Introduction: The Phonetic Basis of Phonological. Dans B. Hayes, R. Kirchner & D. Steriade (Eds.), Phonetically-Based Phonology (pp. 1-32). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  71. Hulst, H. v. d. (1984). Syllable structure and stress in Dutch. Dordrect: Foris.
  72. Hume, E. (2006). Language Specific and Universal Markedness: An Information-theoretic Approach. Communication présentée à LSA Annual Meeting, Colloquium on Information Theory and Phonology.
  73. Hyman, L. (1985). A Theory of Phonological Weight (Vol. 19). Dordrecht: Foris.
  74. Inkelas, S. et Rose, Y. (2003). Velar Fronting Revisited. Communication présentée à Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Somerville, MA.
  75. Inkelas, S. et Rose, Y. (à paraître). Positional Neutralization: A Case Study from Child Language. Language.
  76. Itô, J. (1986). Syllable Theory in Prosodic Phonology. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
  77. Jakobson, R. (1962). Typological Studies and their Contribution to Historical Comparative Linguistics: Report in the First Plenary Session of the Eighth International Congress of Linguists, Oslo 5 August 1957. Dans R. H. Whitman (Ed.), Selected Writings 1: Phonological Studies. The Hague: Mouton.
  78. Jakobson, R. (1969). Langage enfantin et aphasie. Paris: Éditions de Minuit.
  79. Jakobson, R., Fant, C. G. M. et Halle, M. (1952). Preliminaries to speech analysis: the distinctive features and their correlates. Cambridge: Acoustics Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
  80. Jakobson, R. et Halle, M. (1956). Fundamentals of Language (S.-Gravenhage, ed.). The Hague: Mouton & Co.
  81. Jespersen, O. (1922). Language. Its Nature, Development and Origin. London: Allen & Unwin.
  82. Jun, J. (2004). A perception-based analysis of place assimilation. Dans B. Hayes, R. Kirchner & S. Donca (Eds.), Phonetically-Based Phonology (pp. 58-86). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  83. Jusczyk, P. W., Cutler, A. et Redanz, N. J. (1993). Infants’ Preference for the Predominant Stress Patterns of English Words. Child Development, 64(3), 675-687.
  84. Kager, R. (1999). Optimality theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  85. Kahn, D. (1976). Syllable-based Generalizations in English Phonology. New Yorg: Garland Press.
  86. Kaye, J. (1990). ‘Coda’ Licensing. Phonology, 7, 301-330.
  87. Kaye, J. (1992). Do you Believe in Magic? The Story of s+C Sequences. SOAS Working Papers in Linguistics, 2, 293-313.
  88. Kaye, J. et Lowenstamm, J. (1984). De la syllabicité. Dans F. Dell, D. Hirst & J.-R. Vergnaud (Eds.), Forme sonore du langage (pp. 123-161). Paris: Hermann.
  89. Kaye, J., Lowenstamm, J. et Vergnaud, J.-R. (1985). The Internal Structure of Phonological Elements: A Theory of Charm and Government. Phonology Yearbook, 2, 305-328.
  90. Kaye, J., Lowenstamm, J. et Vergnaud, J.-R. (1990). Constituent Structure and Government Phonology. Phonology, 7, 193-231.
  91. Kenstowicz, M. (1994). Phonology in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
  92. Kent, R. D. (1981). Articulatory-Acoustic Perspective on Speech Development. Dans Rachel E. Stark (Ed.), Language Behavior in Infancy and Early Childhood (pp. 105-126). New York: Elsevier.
  93. Kewley-Port, D. et Preston, M. S. (1974). Early apical stop production: A voice onset time analysis. Journal of Phonetics, 2, 195-210.
  94. Kirk, C. et Demuth, K. (2003). Onset/coda asymmetries in the acquisition of clusters. Communication présentée à 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development.
  95. Klatt, D. H. (1975). Voice onset time, frication, and aspiration in word-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 18(4), 686-706.
  96. LaBelle, C. L. (2000). A longitudinal study of lexical and prosodic differentiation by a simultaneous French/English bilingual child (1;5-2;3). Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development, 24, 474-485.
  97. Ladefoged, P. et Maddieson, I. (1996). The Sounds of the World’s Languages. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
  98. Levin, J. (1985). A Metrical Theory of Syllabicity. Ph.D. Dissertation, MIT.
  99. Lisker, L. et Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384-422.
  100. Lisker, L. et Abramson, A. S. (1967). Some effects of context on voice onset time in English stops. Language and Speech, 10(1), 1-28.
  101. Lombardi, L. (1999). Positional faithfulness and voicing assimilation in Optimality Theory. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 17(2), 267-302.
  102. Lyche, C. (2005). Des règles aux contraintes : quelques aspects de la théorie de l’optimalité. Dans N. Nguyen, S. Wauquier-Gravelines & J. Durand (Eds.), Phonologie et phonétique : forme et substance (pp. 209-264). Paris: Hermes Science Publications.
  103. Macken, M. A. (1996). Prosodic constraints on features. Dans Proceedings of the UBC International Conference on Phonological Acquisition (pp. 159-172). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
  104. Macken, M. A. et Ferguson, C. (1983). Cognitive Aspects of Phonological Development: Model, Evidence and Issues. Dans Keith E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s Language (Vol. 4, pp. 255-282). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  105. Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for Things: A Study of Child Language. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books / MIT Press.
  106. MacNeilage, P. F. et Davis, B. (1990). Acquisition of Speech Production: Frames, Then Content. Dans M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance XIII. Motor Representation and Control (pp. 453-475). Hillsdale, New Jersey, Hove and London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  107. MacNeilage, P. F. et Davis, B. L. (1999). Evolution of the form of spoken words. Evolution of Communication, 3(1), 3-20.
  108. MacWhinney, B. (1991). The CHILDES Project : Tools for Analyzing Talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  109. Maddieson, I. (1984). Patterns of Sounds. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  110. Maddieson, I. et Precoda, K. (1990). Updating UPSID. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, 74, 104-114.
  111. Malécot, A. (1974). Frequency of occurence of French phonemes and consonant. Phonetica, 29, 158-170.
  112. Martinet, A. (2005). Économie des changements phonétiques : traité de phonologie diachronique. Paris: Maisonneuve & Larose.
  113. Mattys, S. L. et Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Phonotactic cues for segmentation of fluent speech by infants. Cognition, 78(2), 91-121.
  114. Maye, J., Werker, J. F. et Gerken, L. A. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82(3), B101–B111.
  115. McCarthy, J. J. (1979). Formal Problems in Semitic Phonology and Morphology. Ph.D. Dissertation, MIT.
  116. McCarthy, J. J. (1997). Faithfulness in Prosodic Morphology and Phonology: Rotuman Revisited. Ms.
  117. McCarthy, J. J. (2002). A thematic guide to Optimality Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  118. McCarthy, J. J. (2003). OT constraints are categorical. Phonology, 20(1), 75-138.
  119. McCarthy, J. J. et Prince, A. S. (1986). Prosodic Morphology. Ms.
  120. McCarthy, J. J. et Prince, A. S. (1995). Faithfulness and Reduplicative Identity. Dans Jill N. Beckman, L. W. Dickey & S. Urbanczyk (Eds.), Papers in Optimality Theory (pp. 249-384). Amherst: GLSA.
  121. McCarthy, J. J. et Prince, A. S. (1997). L’émergence du non-marqué : l’optimalité en morphologie prosodique. Langages, 31(125), 55-87.
  122. Ménard, L. (2002). Production et perception des voyelles au cours de la croissance du conduit vocal : variabilité, invariance et normalisation. Thèse de doctorat : Sciences du langage, Université Stendhal Grenoble 3.
  123. Menn, L. (1976). Pattern, Control, and Contrast in Beginning Speech: A Case Study in the Development of Word Form and Word Function. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana Champaign.
  124. Menn, L. (1983). Development of articulatory, phonetic, and phonological capabilities. Dans B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production: Development, writing, and other language processes (Vol. 2, pp. 1-50). London: Academic Press.
  125. Miller, W. R. (1965). Acoma grammar and texts. Berkeley: University of California Press.
  126. Monsen, R. B. (1976). The Production of English Stop Consonants in the Speech of Deaf Children. Journal of Phonetics, 4(1), 29-41.
  127. Morrisette, M. L., Dinnsen, D. A. et Gierut, J. A. (2003). Markedness and context effects in the acquisition of place features. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique, 48(3/4), 329-356.
  128. New, B. (2006). Lexique 3 : Une nouvelle base de données lexicales. Communication présentée aux Actes de la Conférence Traitement Automatique des Langues Naturelles (TALN 2006), Avril 2006, Louvain, Belgique.
  129. Ohala, J. J. (1983). The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract constraints. Dans P. F. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 189 - 216). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  130. Ohala, J. J. (1994). Speech aerodynamics. Dans R. E. Asher & J. M. Y. Simpson (Eds.), The Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (pp. 4144-4148). Oxford: Pergamon.
  131. Ohala, J. J. (1997, 11-15 Aug). Aerodynamics of phonology. Communication présentée à 4th Seoul International Conference on Linguistics [SICOL], Seoul, Korea.
  132. Ohala, J. J. et Riordan, C. J. (1979). Passive vocal tract enlargement during voiced stops, New York.
  133. Ooijen, B. v., Bertoncini, J., Sansavini, A. et Mehler, J. (1997). Do weak syllables count for newborns? The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(6), 3735-3741.
  134. Paradis, C. (1993). Ill-Formedness in the Dictionary: A Source of Constraint Violation. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique, 38(2), 215-234.
  135. Paradis, C. et Prunet, J.-F. (Eds.). (1991). The Special Status of Coronals: Internal and External Evidence (Vol. 2). San Diego: Academic Press.
  136. Paradis, J., Petitclerc, S. et Genesee, F. (1997). Word Truncation in French-speaking Two-year-olds. Communication présentée à Proceedings of the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Somerville, MA.
  137. Pater, J. (1997). Minimal Violation and Phonological Development. Language Acquisition, 6(3), 201-253.
  138. Pater, J. (2000). Non-uniformity in English secondary stress: The role of ranked and lexically specific constraints. Phonology, 17(2), 237-274.
  139. Pater, J. et Barlow, J. A. (2003). Constraint conflict in cluster reduction. Journal of Child Language, 30(3), 487-526.
  140. Pater, J. et Werle, A. (2003). Direction of assimilation in child consonant harmony. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique, 48(3/4), 385-408.
  141. Piggott, Glyne L. (1996). Reconfiguring Harmony. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics, 12(1), 61-98.
  142. Piggott, Glyne L. (1997). Licensing and Alignment: A Conspiracy in Harmony. Phonology, 14, 437-477.
  143. Piggott, G. L. (1999). At the Right Edge of Words. The Linguistic Review, 16(2), 143-185.
  144. Piggott, Glyne L. (2000). Against Featural Alignment. Journal of Linguistics, 36, 85-129.
  145. Pike, K. et Pike, E. (1947). Immediate Constituents of Mazateco Syllables. International Journal of American Linguistics, 13, 78-91.
  146. Pinker, S. (1984). Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  147. Preston, M. S., Yeni-Komshian, G. H. et Stark, R. E. (1967). A study of voicing in initial stops found in the pre-linguistic vocalizations of infants from different language environments. Dans Status Report on Speech Research, SR-10 (pp. 109-120). New York: Haskins Laboratories.
  148. Prince, A. et Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar: Rutgers University and University of Colorado, Boulder.
  149. Raphael, Lawrence J. (2005). Acoustic cues to the perception of segmental phonemes. Dans David B. Pisoni & Robert E. Remez (Eds.), The handbook of speech perception (pp. 182-206). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.
  150. Redford, M. A. (1999). An articulatory basis for the syllable. University of Texas, Austin.
  151. Rose, Y. (1999a). Licensing and Head-dependent Relations in Explaining Partial Deletions in Loanword Phonology. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics, 14, 155-192.
  152. Rose, Y. (1999b). Perception, Representation, and Correspondence Relations in Loanword Phonology. Communication présentée à Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley.
  153. Rose, Y. (1999c, June). Quand la structure métrique impose son accent au trait harmonique.Communication présentée à 1999 Annual Meeting of the Canadian Linguistic Association, Université de Sherbrooke.
  154. Rose, Y. (2000). Headedness and Prosodic Licensing in the L1 Acquisition of Phonology.Ph.D. Dissertation, McGill University.
  155. Rose, Y. (2003). Place Specification and Segmental Distribution in the Acquisition of Word-final Consonant Syllabification. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique, 48(3/4), 409-435.
  156. Rose, Y. et Champdoizeau, C. (2007). Debunking the Trochaic Bias Myth: Evidence from Phonological Development. Communication présentée à 33rd Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, University of California, Berkeley.
  157. Rose, Y. et dos Santos, C. (2005, 8-10 décembre). Sonorants acquisition and positional neutralization: typological, prosodic and articulatory considerations. Communication présentée à ELA 2005, Lyon, France.
  158. Rose et dos Santos, C. (2006). Facteurs prosodiques et articulatoires dans l’harmonie consonantique et la métathèse en acquisition du français langue première. Recherches linguistiques de Vincennes, 35, 77-102.
  159. Rousset, I. (2004). Structures syllabiques et lexicales des langues du monde : données, typologies, tendances universelles et contraintes substancielles. Thèse de doctorat : Sciences du langage, Université Stendhal Grenoble 3.
  160. Sagey, E. (1986). The Representation of Features and Relations in Non-Linear Phonology. Ph.D. Dissertation, MIT.
  161. Schane, S. (1987). The Resolution of Hiatus. Communication présentée à 23rd Annual Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Part two: Parasession on Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology, Chicago.
  162. Scheer, T. (2004). A lateral theory of phonology : what is CVCV, and why should it be?Berlin ; New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
  163. Selkirk, Elizabeth O. (1978). The French foot: On the status of "mute e". Studies in French Linguistics, 1(2), 141-155.
  164. Selkirk, Elisabeth O. (1980a). The Role of Prosodic Categories in English Word Stress. Linguistic Inquiry, 11, 563-605.
  165. Selkirk, Elisabeth O. (1980b). Prosodic Domains in Phonology: Sanskrit Revisited. Dans M. Aronoff & M.-L. Kean (Eds.), Juncture. A Collection of Original Papers (pp. 107-129). Saratoga, CA: Anma Libri.
  166. Selkirk, Elizabeth O. (1982). The Syllable. Dans H. v. d. Hulst & N. Smith (Eds.), The Structure of Phonological Representation (Vol. 2, pp. 337-385). Dordrecht: Foris.
  167. Selkirk, Elizabeth O. (1984). On the Major Class Features and Syllable Theory. Dans M. Aronoff & Richard T. Oehrle (Eds.), Language and Sound Structure (pp. 107-136). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  168. Serniclaes, W. (1984). Fenêtre de prélèvement temporel des indices. Dans Actes des 13èmes J.E.P. (pp. 69-78). Bruxelles.
  169. Serniclaes, W. (1987). Etude expérimentale de la perception du trait de voisement des occlusives du français.Université Libre de Bruxelles.
  170. Smith, N. V. (1973). The Acquisition of Phonology, a Case Study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  171. Smolensky, P. (1996). On the Comprehension/Production Dilemma in Child Language. Linguistic Inquiry, 27, 720-731.
  172. Stampe, D. L. (1969). The Acquisition of Phonetic Representation. Communication présentée à Papers from the 5th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Chicago.
  173. Stampe, D. (1979). A dissertation on natural phonology. New York: Garland.
  174. Steriade, D. (1982). Greek Prosodies in the Nature of Syllabification.Ph.D. Dissertation, MIT.
  175. Stoel-Gammon, C. (1985). Phonetic Inventories, 15-24 months: A Longitudinal Study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 505-512.
  176. Stoel-Gammon, C. et Cooper, J. A. (1984). Patterns of Early Lexical and Phonological Development. Journal of Child Language, 11, 247-271.
  177. Studdert-Kennedy, M. et Goodell, E. W. (1993). Acoustic evidence for the development of gestural co-ordination in the speech of 2-year-olds: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36(4), 707-727.
  178. Tranel, B. (1981). Concreteness in Generative Phonology: Evidence from Modern French. Berkeley: University of California Press.
  179. Tranel, B. (2000). Aspect de la phonologie du français et la théorie de l’optimalité. Langue française, 126, 39–72.
  180. Tuller, B. et Kelso, J. A. S. (1990). Phase Transitions in Speech Production and Their Perceptual Consequences. Dans E. b. M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and Performance XIII. Motor Representation and Control (pp. 429-452). Hillsdale, New Jersey, Hove and London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  181. Tuller, B. et Kelso, J. A. S. (1991). The production and perception of syllable structure. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34(3), 501-508.
  182. Turk, A. E., Jusczyk, P. W. et Gerken, L. (1995). Do English-learning infants use syllable weight to determine stress? Language and Speech, 38, 143-158.
  183. Velleman, Shelly S. (1996). Metathesis Highlights Feature-by-position constraints. Dans Barbara H. Bernhardt, J. Gilbert & D. Ingram (Eds.), Proceedings of the UBC International Conference on Phonological Acquisition (pp. 173-186). Somerville: Cascadilla Press.
  184. Vennemann, T. (1982). Zur Silbenstruktur der deutschen Standardsprache. Dans T. Vennemann (Ed.), Silben, Segmente, Akzente (pp. 261−305). Tübingen: Niemeyer.
  185. Vihnam, M. (1996). Phonological development : the origins of language in the child. Cambridge (MA): Blackwell.
  186. Walsh Dickey, L. (1997). The Phonology of Liquids. Amherst: University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
  187. Waterson, N. (1971). Child phonology: A prosodic view. Journal of Linguistics, 7(2), 179-211.
  188. Wiese, R. (1996). The Phonology of German. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  189. Wiese, R. (2001). The phonology of /r. Dans T. A. Hall (Ed.), Distinctive Feature Theory (pp. 335-368). Berlin: de Gruyter.
  190. Zamuner, T. S. (2003). Input-based phonological acquisition. New York: Routledge.
  191. Zec, D. (1988). Sonority Constraints on Prosodic Structure. Ph.D. Dissertation, Stanford University.