Bibliographie

  1. Abramson, A. S., & Lisker, L. (1970). Discriminability along the voicing continuum: Cross-language tests. Paper presented at the Sixth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Prague: Academia.
  2. Adlard, A., & Hazan, V. (1998). Speech perception in children with specific reading difficulties (dyslexia). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 51, 153-177.
  3. Ahissar, M., Protopapas, A., Reid, M., & Merzenich, M. M. (2000). Auditory processing parallels reading abilities in adults. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 97, 6832–6837.
  4. AmericanPsychiatricAssociation. (2004). DSM-IV-TR : manuel diagnostique et statistique des troubles mentaux. Paris: Editions Masson.
  5. Andruski, J. E., Blumstein, S. E., & Burton, M. (1994). The effect of subphonetic differences on lexical access. Cognition, 52, 163-187.
  6. Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry—The methods. NeuroImage, 11, 805-821.
  7. Axelsson, A., Jerson, T., Lindberg, U., & Lindgren, F. (1981). Early noise-induced hearing loss in teenage boys. Scandinavian Audiology. Supplementum, 10, 91-96.
  8. Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  9. Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, in press.
  10. Baddeley, L. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.
  11. Bailey, P. J., & Snowling, M. J. (2002). Auditory processing and the development of language and literacy. British Medical Bulletin, 63, 135-146.
  12. Banai, K., & Kraus, N. (2006). Neurobiology of (central) auditory processing disorder and language-bases learning disability. In G. D. Chermak & F. E. Musiek (Eds.), Handbook of Central Auditory Processing Disorder: Volume I: Auditory Neuroscience and Diagnosis (pp. 89-116). San Diego: Plural Publishing Inc.
  13. Banai, K., Nicol, T., Zecker, S. G., & Kraus, N. (2005). Brainstem timing: Implications for cortical processing and literacy. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 9850-9857.
  14. Banel, M.-H., & Bacri, N. (1997). Reconnaissance de la parole et indices de segmentation métriques et phonotactiques. L'Année psychologique, 97, 77-112.
  15. Bear, M. F., Connors, B. W., & Paradiso, M. A. (1999). Neuroscience, à la découverte du cerveau: Pradel.
  16. Beaton, A. A., McDougall, S., & Singleton, C. (1997). Editorial humpty dumpty grows up? - Diagnosing dyslexia in adulthood. Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 1-6.
  17. Bedoin, N. (2003). Sensitivity to voicing similarity in printed stimuli: Effect of a training programme in dyslexic children. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 541-546.
  18. Bedoin, N., & Krifi, S. (à paraître). The complexity of phonetic features origanisation in reading. In F. Pellegrino, E. Marsico, I. Chitoran & C. Coupé (Eds.), Approaches to phonological complexity: Mouton and de Gruyter.
  19. Békésy, G. (1960). Experiments in hearing. New-York: McGraw-Hill.
  20. Berent, I., & Perfetti, C. A. (1995). A rose is a REEZ: The two-cycles model of phonology assembly in reading english. Psychological Review, 102, 146-184.
  21. Bertelson, P., Morais, J., Alegria, J., & Content, A. (1985). Phonetic analysis capacity and learning to read. Nature, 313, 73-74.
  22. Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Cox, R. W., Rao, S. M., & Prieto, T. (1997). Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 353-362.
  23. Blumstein, S. E., & Stevens, K. N. (1979). Acoustic invariance in speech production: Evidence from measurements of the spectral characteristics of stop consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66, 1001-1017.
  24. Boder, E. (1973). Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach based on three atypical reading-spelling patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15, 663-687.
  25. Bogliotti, C., Serniclaes, W., Messaoud-Galusi, S., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (in press). Discrimination of speech sounds by children with dyslexia: Comparisons with chronological age and reading level controls. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.
  26. Bosse, M.-L., & Valdois, S. (2002). Phonological and visual processing in dyslexic children: A group study (Traitements phonologiques et visuels chez l'enfant dyslexique : une étude de groupe). Paper presented at the Annual meeting of the Société de Neuropsychologie de Langue Française, Paris.
  27. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorising sounds and learning to read: A causal connexion. Nature, 301, 419-421.
  28. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. International academy for research in learning disabilities monograph series No. 1. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
  29. Bradlow, A. R., Kraus, N., & Hayes, E. (2003). Speaking clearly for children with learning disabilities: Sentence perception in noise. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 80-97.
  30. Bradlow, A. R., Kraus, N., Nicol, T. G., McGee, T. J., Cunningham, J., Zecker, S. G., et al. (1999). Effects of lengthened formant transition duration on discrimination and neural representation of synthetic CV syllables by normal and learning-disabled children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 2086-2096.
  31. Brady, S., Shankweiler, D. P., & Mann, V. (1983). Speech perception and memory coding in relation to reading ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 345-367.
  32. Brambati, S. M., Termine, C., Ruffino, M., Stella, G., Fazio, F., Cappa, S. F., et al. (2004). Regional reductions of gray matter volume in familial dyslexia. Neurology, 63, 742-745.
  33. Bray, P., & Kemp, D. T. (1987). An advanced cochlear echo technique suitable for infant screening. British Journal of Audiology, 21, 191-204.
  34. Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound. Cambridge: Bradford Books
  35. MIT Press.
  36. Broca, P. (1865). Sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé [On the localization of the faculty of articulated language]. Bulletin of the Society of Anthropology, 6, 377-396.
  37. Brown, J. I., Fishco, V. V., & Hanna, G. S. (1993). Nelson-Denny reading test: Manual for scoring and interpretation, forms G & H. Itasca, Illinois: Riverside Publishing.
  38. Brown, W. E., Eliez, S., Menon, V., Rumsey, J. M., White, C. D., & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Preliminary evidence of widespread morphological variations of the brain in dyslexia. Neurology, 56, 781-783.
  39. Brownell, W. E. (1983). Observations on a motile response in isolated outer hair cells. In W. R. Webster & L. M. Aitken (Eds.), Mechanisms of Hearing (pp. 5-10): Monash University Press.
  40. Brownell, W. E. (1990). Outer hair cell electromotility and otoacoustic emissions. Ear and Hearing, 11, 82-92.
  41. Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439-454.
  42. Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874-886.
  43. Bruck, M. (1998). Outcomes of adults with childhood histories of dyslexia. In C. Hulme & M. Joshi (Eds.), Cognitive and linguistic bases of reading, writing, and spelling (pp. 179-200). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  44. Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1990). Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: The case of initial consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 156-178.
  45. Brunswick, N., & Rippon, G. (1994). Auditory event- related potentials, dichotic listening performance and handedness as indices of lateralisation in dyslexic and normal readers. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 265-275.
  46. Brysbaert, M. (2007). "The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy": Some simple SPSS solutions to a complex problem: Royal Holloway, University of London.
  47. Burns, E. M., Arehart, K. H., & Campbell, S. L. (1992). Prevalence of spontaneous otoacoustic emissions in neonates. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91, 1571-1575.
  48. Calliope. (1989). La parole et son traitement automatique. In Masson (Ed.).
  49. Cardon, L. R., Smith, S. D., Fulker, D. W., Kimberling, W. J., Pennington, B. F., & DeFries, J. C. (1994). Quantitative trait locus for reading disability on chromosome 6. Science, 266, 276–279.
  50. Carlyon, R. P. (2004). How the brain separates sounds. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 465-471.
  51. Carré, R., & Chennoukh, S. (1995). Vowel-consonant-vowel modeling by superposition of consonant closure on vowel-to-vowel gestures. Journal of Phonetics, 23, 231-241.
  52. Carré, R., Serniclaes, W., & Marsico, E. (2003). Formant transition duration versus prevoicing duration in voiced stop identification. Paper presented at the 15th ICPhS, Barcelone.
  53. Casagrande, J. (1984). The sound system of french. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
  54. Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149-180.
  55. Cazals, Y. (1994). Occlusive silence duration of voiceless intervocalic plosives and voicing perception by normal and hearing-impaired subjects. Ear and Hearing, 15, 404-408.
  56. Charpentier, F. J., & Stella, M. G. (1986). Diphone synthesis using an overlap-add technique for speech waveforms concatenation. Paper presented at the IEEE International Conference ASSP, Tokyo.
  57. Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one or two ears. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975-979.
  58. Chéry-Croze, S., Moulin, A., Collet, L., & Morgon, A. (1994). Is the test of medial efferent system function a relevant investigation in tinnutus? British Journal of Audiology, 28, 13-25.
  59. Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 335-359.
  60. Collet, L., Kemp, D. T., Veuillet, E., Duclaux, R., Moulin, A., & Morgon, A. (1990). Effect of contralateral auditory stimuli on active cochlear micro-mechanical properties in human subjects. Hearing Research, 43, 251-261.
  61. Collet, L., Levy, V., Veuillet, E., Truy, E., & Morgon, A. (1993). Click-evoked otoacoustic emissions and hearing threshold in sensorineural hearing loss. Ear & Hearing, 14, 141-143.
  62. Collet, L., Rogé, B., Descouens, D., Moron, P., Duverdy, F., & Urgell, H. (1993). Objective auditory dysfunction in infantile autism. Lancet, 342, 923-924.
  63. Collet, L., Veuillet, E., Bene, J., & Morgon, A. (1992). Effects of contralateral white noise on click-evoked emissions in normal and sensorineural ears: Towards an exploration of the medial olivocochlear system. Audiology, 31, 1-7.
  64. Collet, L., Veuillet, E., Moulin, A., Morlet, T., Giraud, A. L., Micheyl, C., et al. (1994). Contralateral auditory stimulation and otoacoustic emissions: A review of basic data in humans. British Journal of Audiology, 28, 213-218.
  65. Colombo, J., & Bundy, R. (1983). Infant response to auditory familiarity and novelty. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 305-311.
  66. Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing. NewYork: Academic Press.
  67. Coltheart, M., Masterson, J., Byng, S., Prior, M., & Riddoch, J. (1983). Surface dyslexia. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 35, 469-495.
  68. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. C. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204-256.
  69. Cornelissen, P. L., Hansen, L., Bradley, L., & Stein, J. F. (1996). Analysis of perceptual confusions between nine sets of consonant-vowel sounds in normal and dyslexic adults. Cognition, 59, 275-306.
  70. Cornelissen, P. L., Richardson, A., Mason, A., Fowler, S., & Stein, J. F. (1995). Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measured at photopic luminance levels in dyslexics and controls. Vision Research, 35, 1483-1494.
  71. Crottaz-Herbette, S., & Ragot, R. (2000). Perception of complex sounds: N1 latency codes pitch and topography codes spectra. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111, 1759-1766.
  72. Cunningham, J., Nicol, T. G., Zecker, S. G., Bradlow, A. R., & Kraus, N. (2001). Neurobiologic responses to speech in noise in children with learning problems: Deficits and strategies for improvement. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 758-767.
  73. Cutler, A., & Norris, D. (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 113-121.
  74. Davis, H. (1983). An active process in cochlear mechanics. Hearing Research, 9, 79-90.
  75. Davis, M. H., & Johnsrude, I. S. (2007). Hearing speech sounds: Top-down influences on the interface between audition and speech perception. Hearing Research, 229, 132-147.
  76. de Bree, E., Wilsenach, C., Gerrits, E., & Wijnen, F. (2003, 1-4 July). Phonological processing and working memory in children at risk for dyslexia and children with SLI. Paper presented at the The International Child Phonology Conference, Vancouver.
  77. De Venecia, R. K., Liberman, M. C., Guinan Jr, J. J., & Brown, M. C. (2005). Medial olivocochlear reflex interneurons are located in the posteroventral cochlear nucleus: A kainic acid lesion study in guinea pigs. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 487, 345-360.
  78. Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Pallier, C., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Jobert, A., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Neural correlates of switching from auditory to speech perception. NeuroImage, 24, 21-33.
  79. Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of mathematical thinking: Behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science, 284, 970-974.
  80. Déjerine, J. (1892). Contribution à l'étude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des différentes variétés de cécité verbale. Mémoires de la Société de Biologie, 4, 61-90.
  81. Delattre, P. C. (1958). Les indices acoustiques de la parole. Premier rapport. Phonetica 2, 108-118, 210-251.
  82. Delattre, P. C. (1959). Rapports entre la durée vocalique, le timbre et la structure syllabique en français. The French Review, 32, 547-552.
  83. Delattre, P. C. (1966a). Studies in French and Comparative Phonetics: selected papers in French and English. Paris, France: The Hague Mouton & Co.
  84. Delattre, P. C. (1966b). Tendances de coupe syllabique en français. In Mouton (Ed.), Studies in French and Comparative Phonetics (pp. 150-162). The Hague.
  85. Delattre, P. C. (1966c). Un triangle acoustique des voyelles du français. In Studies in French and comparative phonetics. La Haye: Mouton.
  86. Delattre, P. C., Liberman, A. M., & Cooper, F. S. (1955). Acoustic loci and transitional cues for consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27, 769-773.
  87. Démonet, J. F., Taylor, M., & Chaix, Y. (2004). Developmental dyslexia. Lancet, 363, 1451-1460.
  88. Démonet, J. F., Thierry, G., & Cardebat, D. (2005). Renewal of the neurophysiology of language: Functional neuroimaging. Physiological Reviews, 85, 49-95.
  89. Devlin, J. T., Raley, J., Tunbridge, E., Lanary, K., Floyer-Lea, A., Narain, C., et al. (2003). Functional asymmetry for auditory processing in human primary auditory cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 11516 -11522.
  90. Dolan, D. F., & Nuttall, A. L. (1988). Masked cochlear whole-nerve response intensity functions altered by electrical stimulation of the crossed olivocochlear bundle. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 83, 1081-1086.
  91. Dorman, M. F., & Raphael, L. J. (1980). Distribution of acoustic cues for stop consonant place of articulation in VCV syllables. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67, 1333-1335.
  92. Dorman, M. F., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Raphael, L. J. (1977). Stop-consonant recognition: Release bursts and formant transitions as functionally equivalent, context-dependent cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 22, 109-122.
  93. Dos Santos, C. (2007). Développement phonologique en français langue maternelle. Une étude de cas. Université Lumière Lyon 2, Lyon, France.
  94. Drullman, R., Festen, J. M., & Plomp, R. (1994). Effect of temporal envelope smearing on speech reception. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95, 1053-1064.
  95. Dufor, O., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Démonet, J.-F. (2007). Top-down processes during auditory phoneme categorization in dyslexia: A PET study. NeuroImage, 34, 1692-1707.
  96. Dumay, N., Content, A., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (1999). Contribution de la structure syllabique de surface à la segmentation lexicale.Paper presented at the 2èmes Journées d’Étude Linguistiques, Nantes.
  97. Dupoux, E., & Green, K. (1997). Perceptual adjustment to highly compressed speech: Effects of talker and rate changes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 914-927.
  98. Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Monitoring the lexicon with normal and compressed speech: Frequency effects and the prelexical code. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 316-335.
  99. Dworczak, F., Bedoin, N., & Krifi, S. (sous presse). Internet et troubles d'apprentissage : les synergies et partenariats créés. In G. Pithon, S. Asdih & C. Larivée (Eds.), Construire une communauté éducative : le partenariat familles - associations - écoles - universités: De Boeck, Bruxelles, De Boeck, Collection Perspectives en éducation et formation.
  100. Ecalle, J., & Magnan, A. (2002). The development of epiphonological and metaphonological processing at the start of learning to read: A longitudinal study. European journal of psychology of education, 17, 47-62.
  101. Ecalle, J., & Magnan, A. (2002). L'apprentissage de la lecture : Fonctionnement et développement cognitifs. Paris: Colin.
  102. Eckert, M. A. (2004). Neuroanatomical markers for dyslexia: A review of dyslexia structural imaging studies. Neuroscientist, 10, 362-371.
  103. Eckert, M. A., & Leonard, C. M. (2000). Structural imaging in dyslexia: The planum temporale. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 6, 198–206.
  104. Eckert, M. A., Leonard, C. M., Wilke, M., Eckert, M., Richards, T., Richards, A., et al. (2005). Anatomical signatures of dyslexia in children: Unique information from manual and voxel based morphometry brain measures. Cortex, 41, 304-315.
  105. Eden, G. F., Jones, K. M., Cappell, K., Gareau, L., Wood, F. B., Zeffiro, T. A., et al. (2004). Neural changes following remediation in adult developmental dyslexia. Neuron, 44, 411-422.
  106. Eden, G. F., VanMeter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., Maisog, J. M., Woods, R. P., & Zeffiro, T. A. (1996). Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature, 382, 66-69.
  107. Edgar, J. C., Yeo, R. A., Gangestad, S. W., Blake, M. B., Davis, J. T., Lewine, J. D., et al. (2006). Reduced auditory M100 asymmetry in schizophrenia and dyslexia: Applying a developmental instability approach to assess atypical brain asymmetry. Neuropsychologia, 44, 289-299.
  108. Eimas, P. D., & Corbit, J. D. (1973). Selective adaptation of linguistic feature detectors. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 99-109.
  109. Eimas, P. D., & Miller, J. L. (1992). Organization in the perception of speech by young infants. Psychological Science, 3, 340-345.
  110. Eimas, P. D., Miller, J. L., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1987). On infant speech perception and the acquisition of language. In S. Harnad (Ed.), Categorical perception: The groundwork of Cognition. New York: Cambridge Univerity Press.
  111. Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P. W., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303-306.
  112. Elbro, C., Nielsen, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: Evidence for deficits in non-word reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 203-226.
  113. Elliott, L. L., Hammer, M. A., & Scholl, M. E. (1989). Fine-grained auditory discrimination in normal children and children with language-learning problems. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 112-119.
  114. Ellis, W. D. (1974). A source book of Gestalt psychology. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
  115. Elman, J. L., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). Exploiting lawful variability in the speech wave. In J. S. Perkell & D. H. Klatt (Eds.), Invariance and variability in speech processes (pp. 360-385). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  116. Eybalin, M. (1993). Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators of the mammalian cochlea. Physiological Reviews, 73, 309-3373.
  117. Fabre, D., & Bedoin, N. (2003). Sensitivity to sonority in Normal readers and dyslexic children. Current Psychology Letters. Special Issue on Language Disorders and Reading Acquisition, 1, [En ligne], published on line on : 27 novembre 2006. URL : http://cpl.revues.org/document2093.html .
  118. Fant, G. (1960). The acoustic theory of speech production.Mouton, La Haye.
  119. Fant, G. (1973). Speech Sounds and Features. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
  120. Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 460-493.
  121. Fawcett, A. J., & Nicolson, R. I. (1999). Performance of dyslexic children on cerebellar and cognitive tests. Journal of Motor Behavior, 31, 68-78.
  122. Fawcett, A. J., Nicolson, R. I., & Dean, P. (1996). Impaired performance of children with dyslexia on a range of cerebellar tasks. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 259-283.
  123. Feldman, E., Levin, B. E., Fleischmann, J., Jallad, B., Kushch, A., Gross-Glenn, K., et al. (1995). Gender differences in the severity of adult familial dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 7, 155-161.
  124. Felton, R. H., Naylor, C. E., & Wood, F. B. (1990). Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics. Brain and Language, 39, 485-497.
  125. Ferrand, L. (2001). Cognition et lecture : Processus de base de la reconnaissance des mots écrits chez l'adulte. Bruxelles: De Boeck Université.
  126. Fiez, J. A., & Petersen, S. E. (1998). Neuroimaging studies of word reading. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 95, 914-921.
  127. Fisher-Jørgensen, E. (1954). Acoustic analysis of stop consonants. Miscellanea Phonetica, 2, 42-59.
  128. Fisher-Jørgensen, E. (1972). Ptk et bdg français en position intervocalique accentuée. In A. Valdman (Ed.), Papers in Linguistics and phonetics: to the memory of Pierre Delattre (pp. 143-200). The Hague: Mouton.
  129. Fisher, S. E., & Francks, C. (2006). Genes, cognition and dyslexia: Learning to read the genome. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 250-257.
  130. Fisher, S. E., Francks, C., Marlow, A. J., MacPhie, I. L., Newbury, D. F., Cardon, L. R., et al. (2002). Independent genome-wide scans identify a chromosome 18 quantitative-trait locus influencing dyslexia. Nature Genetics, 30, 86–91.
  131. Fisher, S. E., Marlow, A. J., Lamb, J., Maestrini, E., Williams, D. F., Richardson, A. J., et al. (1999). A quantitative-trait locus on chromosome 6p influences different aspects of developmental dyslexia. American Journal of Human Genetics, 64, 146-156.
  132. Fletcher, H. (1929). Speech and Hearing. Princeton, NJ: Von Nostrand Reinhold.
  133. Flynn, J. M., & Rahbar, M. H. (1994). Prevalence of reading failure in boys compared with girls. Psychology in the Schools, 31, 66-71.
  134. Forrest, K., & Morrisette, M. L. (1999). Feature analysis of segmental errors in children with phonological disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 187-194.
  135. Forster, K. I. (1976). Accessing the mental lexicon. In R. J. Wales & E. C. T. Walker (Eds.), New approaches to language mechanisms (pp. 257-287). North-Holland, Amsterdam.
  136. Foulke, E. (1971). The perception of time compressed speech. In D. L. Horton & J. J. Jenkins (Eds.), The perception of language (pp. 79-107). Columbus, OH: Merrill.
  137. Foulke, E., & Sticht, T. G. (1969). Review of research on the intelligibility and comprehension of accelerated speech. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 50-62.
  138. Fowler, C. A. (1986). An event approach to the study of speech perception from a direct-realist perspective. Journal of Phonetics, 14, 3-28.
  139. Fowler, C. A. (1996). Listeners do hear sounds, not tongues. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99, 1730-1741.
  140. Frauenfelder, U. H., & Tyler, L. K. (1987). The process of spoken word recognition: An introduction. Cognition, 25, 1-20.
  141. French, N. R., & Steinberg, J. C. (1947). Factors governing the intelligibility of speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 19, 90-119.
  142. Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Cognitive and neuropsychological studies of phonological reading (pp. 301-330). London: Erlbaum.
  143. Frith, U. (1999). Paradoxes in the definition of dyslexia. Dyslexia, 5, 192-214.
  144. Froehlich, P., Collet, L., Valatx, J. L., & Morgon, A. (1993). Sleep and active cochlear micromechanical properties in human subjects. Hearing Research, 66, 1-7.
  145. Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71-99.
  146. Galaburda, A. M. (1993). Neurology of developmental dyslexia. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 3, 237-242.
  147. Galaburda, A. M., & Kemper, T. L. (1979). Cytoarchitectonic abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: A case study. Annals of Neurology, 6, 94–100.
  148. Galaburda, A. M., & Livingstone, M. S. (1993). Evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 682, 70-82.
  149. Galaburda, A. M., Menard, M. T., & Rosen, G. D. (1994). Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 91, 8010–8013.
  150. Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., Rosen, G. D., Aboitiz, F., & Geschwind, N. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18, 222–233.
  151. Garvey, W. D. (1953). The intelligibility of speeded speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 102-108.
  152. Gauthier, L., Dehaut, F., & Joanette, Y. (1989). The bells test: a quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. The International journal of clinical neuropsychology, 11, 49-54.
  153. Gay, T. (1978). Effect of speaking rate on vowel formant movements. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63, 223-230.
  154. Gayan, J., Smith, S. D., Cherny, S. S., Cardon, L. R., Fulker, D. W., Brower, A. M., et al. (1999). Quantitative-trait locus for specific language and reading deficits on chromosome 6p. American Journal of Human Genetics, 64, 157–164.
  155. Gelfand, S. (2001). Auditory System and Related Disorders. Essentials of Audiology (Second Edition ed.). New York: Thieme.
  156. Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain, 88, 237-294.
  157. Geschwind, N. (1979). Specializations of the human brain. Scientific American, 241, 180-199.
  158. Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A. M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization. Biological mechanisms, associations, and pathology: II. A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of Neurology, 42, 521–552.
  159. Geschwind, N., & Levitsky, W. (1968). Human brain: Left-right asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science, 161, 186-187.
  160. Giard, M.-H., Collet, L., Bouchet, P., & Pernier, J. (1994). Auditory selective attention in the human cochlea. Brain Research, 633, 353-356.
  161. Giraud, A. L., Collet, L., Chéry-Croze, S., Magnan, J., & Chays, A. (1995). Evidence of a medial olivocochlear involvement in contralateral suppression of otoacoustic emissions in humans. Brain Research, 705, 15-23.
  162. Giraud, A. L., Garnier, S., Micheyl, C., Lina, G., Chays, A., & Chéry-Croze, S. (1997). Auditory efferents involved in speech-in-noise intelligibility. NeuroReport, 8, 1779-1783.
  163. Giraud, K., Démonet, J. F., Habib, M., Marquis, P., Chauvel, P., & Liégeois-Chauvel, C. (2005). Auditory evoked potential patterns to voiced and voiceless speech sounds in adult developmental dyslexics with persistent deficits. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1524-1534.
  164. Glorig, A. (1958). A report of two normal hearing studies. Annals Of Otology Rhinology And Laryngology, 67, 93-111.
  165. Godfrey, J., Syrdal-Lasky, A., Millay, K., & Knox, C. (1981). Performance of dyslexic children on speech perception tests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 401-424.
  166. Gombert, J. E. (1990). Le développement des capacités métalinguistiques. Paris: P.U.F.
  167. Gombert, J. E., & Colé, P. (2000). Activités métalinguistiques, lecture et illettrisme. In M. Kail & M. Fayol (Eds.), L'acquisition du langage: Le langage en développement au delà de 3 ans (Vol. 2, pp. 117-150). Paris: PUF.
  168. Gottfried, T. L., Miller, J. L., & Payton, P. E. (1990). Effect of speaking rate on the perception of vowels. Phonetica, 47, 155-172.
  169. Gow Jr., D. W. (2001). Assimilation and anticipation in continuous spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 133-159.
  170. Gow Jr., D. W. (2003). Feature parsing: Feature cue mapping in spoken word recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 575-590.
  171. Grataloup, C. (2007). La reconstruction cognitive de la parole dégradée : étude de l'intelligibilité comme indice d'une capacité cognitive humaine. Université Lumière Lyon 2 & Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, Lyon, France.
  172. Green, K. P., & Miller, J. L. (1985). On the role of visual rate information in phonetic perception. Perception & psychophysics, 38, 269-276.
  173. Greenberg, S. (1999). Speaking in shorthand - A syllable-centric perspective for understanding pronunciation variation. Speech Communication, 29, 159-176.
  174. Greenberg, S., & Arai, T. (1998). Speech intelligibility is highly tolerant of cross-channel spectral asynchrony. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 103, 3057.
  175. Greisbach, R. (1992). Reading aloud at maximal speed. Speech Communication, 11, 469-473.
  176. Groff, J. A., & Liberman, M. C. (2003). Modulation of cochlear afferent response by the lateral olivocochlear system: Activation via electrical stimulation of the inferior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90, 3178-3200.
  177. Guinan Jr., J. J. (1996). The physiology of olivocochlear efferents. In P. J. Dallos, A. N. Popper & R. R. Fay (Eds.), The Cochlea (pp. 435-502). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  178. Habib, M. (1997). Dyslexie : le cerveau singulier. Marseille: Solal.
  179. Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: an overview and working hypothesis. Brain, 123, 2373–2399.
  180. Habib, M., Gayraud, D., Oliva, A., Regis, J., Salamon, G., & Khalil, R. (1991). Effects of handedness and sex on the morphology of the corpus callosum: a study with brain magnetic resonance imaging. Brain and Cognition, 16, 41-61.
  181. Hannula-Jouppi, K., Kaminen-Ahola, N., Taipale, M., Eklund, R., Nopola-Hemmi, J., Kääriäinen, H., et al. (2005). The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental dyslexia. PLoS Genetics, 1, 467-674.
  182. Hardcastle, W. (1973). Some observations on the tense-lax distinction in initial stops in Korean. Journal of Phonetics, 1, 263-272.
  183. Hari, R., & Kiesilä, P. (1996). Deficit of temporal auditory processing in dyslexic adults. Neuroscience Letters, 205, 138-140.
  184. Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 525-532.
  185. Hari, R., Sääskilahti, A., Helenius, P., & Uutela, K. (1999). Non-impaired auditory phase locking in dyslexic adults. NeuroReport, 10, 2347-2348.
  186. Hawkins, S. (2003). Roles and representations of systematic fine phonetic detail in speech understanding. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 373-405.
  187. Hayes, E., Warrier, C., Nicol, T., Zecker, S., & Kraus, N. (2003). Neural plasticity following auditory training in children with learning problems. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 673-684.
  188. Hazan, V., & Rosen, S. (1991). Individual variability in the perception of cues to place contrasts in initial stops. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 187-200.
  189. Heath, S., Hogben, J., & Clark, C. (1999). Auditory temporal processing in disabled readers with and without oral language delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 637-647.
  190. Hécaen, H., & Sauguet, J. (1971). Cerebral dominance in left-handed subjects. Cortex, 7, 19-48.
  191. Heilman, K. M., Voeller, K., & Alexander, A. W. (1996). Developmental dyslexia: a motor-articulatory feedback hypothesis. Annales of Neurology, 39, 407-412.
  192. Heim, S., Kissler, J., Elbert, T., & Rockstroh, B. (2004). Cerebral lateralization in schizophrenia and dyslexia: Neuromagnetic responses to auditory stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 42, 692-697.
  193. Helenius, P., Uutela, K., & Hari, R. (1999). Auditory stream segregation in dyslexic adults. Brain, 122, 907-913.
  194. Herman, A. E., Galaburda, A. M., Fitch, R. H., Carter, A. R., & Rosen, G. D. (1997). Cerebral microgyria, thalamic cell size and auditory temporal processing in male and female rats. Cerebral Cortex, 7, 453-464.
  195. Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 131-138.
  196. Hienz, R., Stiles, P., & May, B. (1998). Effects of bilateral olivocochlear lesions on vowel formant discrimination in cats. Hearing Research, 116, 10-20.
  197. Hill, N. I., Bailey, P. J., Griffiths, Y. M., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Frequency acuity and binaural masking release in dyslexic listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, L53-L58.
  198. Hoeft, F., Meyler, A., Hernandez, A., Juel, C., Taylor-Hill, H., Martindale, J., et al. (2007). Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 104, 4234-4239.
  199. Houtgast, T., & Steeneken, H. J. M. (1985). A review of the MTF concept in room acoustics and its use for estimating speech intelligibility in auditoria. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 77, 1069-1077.
  200. Huffman, R., & Henson, O. (1990). The descending auditory pathway and acousticomotor systems: connections with the inferior colliculus. Brain Research Reviews, 15, 295-323.
  201. Hugdahl, K., Synnevag, B., & Satz, P. (1990). Immune and autoimmune diseases in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia, 28, 673-679.
  202. Humphreys, P., Kaufmann, W. E., & Galaburda, A. M. (1990). Developmental dyslexia in women: Neuropathological findings in three patients. Annals of Neurology, 28, 727-738.
  203. Hynd, G. W., Hall, J., Novey, E. S., Eliopulos, D., Black, K., Gonzales, J. J., et al. (1995). Dyslexia and corpus callosum morphology. Archives of Neurology, 52, 32-38.
  204. Jacquier-Roux, M., Valdois, S., & Zorman, M. (2002). ODEDYS: un outil de dépistage des dyslexies Version 1. Grenoble: Laboratoire cogni-sciences, IUFM de Grenoble.
  205. Jacquier-Roux, M., Valdois, S., Zorman, M., Lequette, C., & Pouget, G. (2005). ODEDYS: un outil de de´pistage des dyslexies Version 2. Grenoble: Laboratoire cogni-sciences, IUFM de Grenoble.
  206. Jakobson, R., Fant, G., & Halle, M. (1952). Preliminaries to speech analysis: the distinctive features and their correlates. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  207. Janse, E. (2003). Production and perception of fast speech.Universiteit Utrecht, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
  208. Jerger, S., Martin, R., & Jerger, J. (1987). Specific auditory perceptual dysfunction in a learning disabled child. Ear and Hearing, 8, 78-86.
  209. Joanisse, M. F., Manis, F. R., Keating, P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2000). Language deficits in dyslexic children: Speech perception, phonology and morphology. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 30-60.
  210. Johnson, K. (1997). Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics Oxford: Blackwell.
  211. Johnson, K. L. (2005). Speaker normalization in speech perception. In D. Pisoni & R. Remez (Eds.), The Handbook of Speech Perception (pp. 363-389). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
  212. Johnsrude, I. S., Zatorre, R. J., Milner, B. A., & Evans, A. C. (1997). Left-hemisphere specialization for the processing of acoustic transients. NeuroReport, 8, 1761-1765.
  213. Josse, G., Hervé, P.-Y., Crivello, F., Mazoyer, B., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2006). Hemispheric specialization for language: Brain volume matters. Brain Research, 1068, 184-193.
  214. Josse, G., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2004). Hemispheric specialization for language. Brain Research Reviews, 44, 1-12.
  215. Jun, J. (1995). Perceptual and Articulatory Factors in Place Assimilation: An Optimality Theoretic Approach. UCLA.
  216. Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). How infants begin to extract words from speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 323-328.
  217. Jusczyk, P. W., & Luce, P. A. (2002). Speech perception and spoken word recognition: Past and present. Ear and Hearing, 23, 2-40.
  218. Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (1994). Infants' sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630-645.
  219. Kakigi, A., Hirakawa, H., Mount, R. J., & Harrison, R. V. (1997). The effects of crossed olivocochlear bundle section on transient evoked otoacoustic emissions. Hearing Research, 110, 34-38.
  220. Kawase, T., Delgutte, B., & Liberman, M. C. (1993). Antimasking effects of the olivocochlear reflex. II. Enhancement of auditory-nerve response to masked tones. Journal of Neurophysiology, 70, 2533-2549.
  221. Kawase, T., & Liberman, M. C. (1993). Antimasking effects of the olivocochlear reflex. I. Enhancement of compound action potentials to masked tones. Journal of Neurophysiology, 70, 2519-2532.
  222. Keller, E., & Zellner, B. (1996). A timing model for fast French. In York Papers in Linguistics (Vol. 17, pp. 53-75): University of York.
  223. Kemp, D. (1978). Stimulated acoustic emissions from within the human auditory system. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 64, 1386-1391.
  224. Kemp, D. (1979). Evidence of mechanical nonlinearity and frequency selective wave amplification in the cochlea. European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology, 224, 37-45.
  225. Kemp, D. (2002). Otoacoustic emissions, their origin in cochlear function, and use. British Medical Bulletin, 63, 223-241.
  226. Kent, R. D., & Moll, K. L. (1969). Vocal-tract characteristics of the stop cognates. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 46, 1549-1555.
  227. Kessinger, R. H., & Blumstein, S. E. (1998). Effects of speaking rate on voice-onset time and vowel production: Some implications for perception studies. Journal of Phonetics, 26, 117-128.
  228. Kewley-Port, D. (1983). Time-varying features as correlates of place of articulation in stop consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73, 322-335.
  229. Khalfa, S. (1999). Caractérisation des asymétries fonctionnelles de l'appareil auditif périphérique chez l'humain : relations avec la spécialisation hémisphérique ? Unpublished Thèse de Doctorat en Neuropsychologie, Université CLaude Bernard - Lyon 1, Lyon.
  230. Khalfa, S., Bougeard, R., Morand, N., Veuillet, E., Isnard, J., Guenot, M., et al. (2001). Evidence of peripheral auditory activity modulation by the auditory cortex in humans. Neuroscience, 104, 347-358.
  231. Khalfa, S., Bruneau, N., Rogé, B., Georgieff, N., Veuillet, E., Adrien, J.-L., et al. (2001). Peripheral auditory asymmetry in infantile autism European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 628-632.
  232. Khalfa, S., & Collet, L. (1996). Functional asymmetry of medial olivocochlear system in humans. Towards a peripheral auditory lateralization. NeuroReport, 7, 993-996.
  233. Khalfa, S., Micheyl, C., Pham, E., Maison, S., Veuillet, E., & Collet, L. (2000). Tones disappear faster in the right ear than in the left. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 647-655.
  234. Khalfa, S., Micheyl, C., Veuillet, E., & Collet, L. (1998). Peripheral auditory lateralization assessment using TEOAEs. Hearing Research, 121, 29-34.
  235. Khalfa, S., Morlet, T., Micheyl, C., Morgon, A., & Collet, L. (1997). Evidence of peripheral hearing asymmetry in humans: clinical implications. Acta Otolaryngologica, 117, 192-196.
  236. Khalfa, S., Veuillet, E., & Collet, L. (1998). Influence of handedness on peripheral auditory asymmetry. European Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 2731-2737.
  237. Kidd, G. R. (1989). Articulatory-rate context effects in phoneme identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 736-748.
  238. King, C., Warrier, C. M., Hayes, E., & Kraus, N. (2002). Deficits in auditory brainstem pathway encoding of speech sounds in children with learning problems. Neuroscience Letters, 319, 111-115.
  239. Klingberg, T., Hedehus, M., Temple, E., Salz, T., Gabrieli, J., Moseley, M., et al. (2000). Microstructure of temporo-parietal white matter as a basis for reading ability: evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuron, 25, 493-500.
  240. Kraus, N. (2001). Auditory pathway encoding and neural plasticity in children with learning problems. Audiology and Neuro-Otology, 6, 221-227.
  241. Kraus, N., McGee, T. J., Carrell, T. D., Zecker, S. G., Nicol, T. G., & Koch, D. B. (1996). Auditory neurophysiologic responses and discrimination deficits in children with learning problems. Science, 273, 971-973.
  242. Krifi, S., Bedoin, N., & Mérigot, A. (2003). Effects of voicing similarity between consonants in printed stimuli, in normal and dyslexic children. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition, 10, 1-7.
  243. Krull, D. (1990). Relating acoustic properties to perceptual responses: A study of Swedish voiced stops. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88, 2557-2570.
  244. Kuhl, P. K. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a "perceptual magnet effect" for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 93-107.
  245. Kujala, T., Myllyviita, K., Tervaniemi, M., Alho, K., Kallio, J., & Näätänen, R. (2000). Basic auditory dysfunction in dyslexia as demonstrated by brain activity measurements. Psychophysiology, 37, 262–266.
  246. Kujala, T., & Näätänen, R. (2001). The mismatch negativity in evaluating central auditory dysfunction in dyslexia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 535-543.
  247. Kumar, U., & Vanaja, C. (2004). Functioning of olivocochlear bundle and speech perception in noise. Ear and Hearing, 25, 142-146.
  248. Ladefoged, P. (2001). Vowels and consonants: an introduction to the sounds of languages. Oxford: Blackwell.
  249. Ladefoged, P., & Broadbent, D. E. (1957). Information Conveyed by Vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 29, 98-104.
  250. Lahiri, A., & Blumstein, S. E. (1981). A reconsideration of acoustic invariance for place of articulation in stop consonants: Evidence from cross-language studies. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Supplement 1(70), S39.
  251. Lahiri, A., & Reetz, H. (2002). Underspecified recognition. In C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (Eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology VII (pp. 637-675). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.
  252. Lalain, M., Demolin, D., Habib, M., Nguyen, N., & Teston, B. (2000). Particularités articulatoires de la dyslexie développementale phonologique. Paper presented at the XXIIIèmes Journées d'Etude de la Parole, Aussois.
  253. Landercy, A., & Renard, R. (1977). Élements de phonétique. Bruxelles: Didier.
  254. Lass, N. J. (1970). The significance of intra- and intersentence pause times in perceptual judgments of oral reading rate. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 13, 777-784.
  255. Lecocq, P. (1991). Apprentissage de la lecture et dyslexie. Bruxelles: Mardaga.
  256. Lecocq, P. (1993). Entraînement à l'analyse segmentale et apprentissage de la lecture. International Journal of Psychology, 28, 549-569.
  257. Lefavrais, P. (1967). Le test de l'Alouette. Paris: Edition du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.
  258. Lefèvre, F. (1985). Une méthode d'analyse auditive des confusions phonétiques : la confrontation indiciaire.Université de Franche-Comté.
  259. Lefly, D., & Pennington, B. (1991). Spelling errors and reading fluency in compensated adult dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 143-162.
  260. Lehiste, I. (1970). Suprasegmentals. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  261. Lehiste, I., & Peterson, G. (1961). Transitions, glides, and diphthongs. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 33, 268-277.
  262. Leonard, C. M., Eckert, M. A., Lombardino, L. J., Oakland, T., Kranzler, J., Mohr, C. M., et al. (2001). Anatomical risk factors for phonological dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 148-157.
  263. Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  264. Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431-461.
  265. Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., & Griffith, B. C. (1957). The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 358-368.
  266. Liberman, A. M., Ingemann, F., Lisker, L., Delattre, P. C., & Cooper, F. S. (1959). Minimal rules for synthesizing speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 31, 1490-1499.
  267. Liberman, A. M., & Mattingly, I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1-36.
  268. Liberman, A. M., & Whalen, D. H. (2000). On the relation of speech to language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 187-196.
  269. Liberman, I. Y. (1973). Segmentation of the spoken word and reading acquisition. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 23, 65-77.
  270. Licklider, J. C. R. (1946). Effects of amplitude distortion upon the intelligibility of speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 18, 429-434.
  271. Licklider, J. C. R. (1950). The intelligibility of amplitude-dichotomized, time-quantized speech waves. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 22, 820-823.
  272. Liénard, J. S. (1972). Analyse, synthèse et reconnaissance de la parole.Université Paris VII, Paris.
  273. Lindblom, B. (1963). Spectrography study of vowel reduction. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 35, 1773-1781.
  274. Lindblom, B. (1990). Explaining phonetic variation: a sketch of the H&H theory. In W. J. Hardcastle & A. Marchal (Eds.), Speech production and speech modelling. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  275. Lindblom, B. E. F., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). On the role of formant transitions in vowel recognition. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 42, 830-843.
  276. Lisker, L. (1986). “Voicing” in English: A catalogue of acoustic features signaling /b/ versus /p/ in trochees. Language and Speech, 29, 3-11.
  277. Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-linguistic study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384-422.
  278. Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1967). Some effects of context on voice onset time in English stops. Language and Speech, 10, 1-28.
  279. Livingstone, M. S., Rosen, G. D., Drislane, F. W., & Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 88, 7943–7947.
  280. Lorenzi, C. (1999). Effets de lésions périphériques et centrales sur l'acuité temporelle auditive. Les Cahiers de l'Audition, 12, 17−30.
  281. Lovegrove, W. J., Bowling, A., Badcock, D., & Blackwood, M. (1980). Specific reading disability: differences in contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency. Science, 210, 439–440.
  282. Lovegrove, W. J., Garzia, R., & Nicholson, S. (1990). Experimental evidence for a transient system deficit in specific reading disability. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 2, 137-146.
  283. Lovegrove, W. J., Martin, F., & Slaghuis, W. (1986). A theoretical and experimental case for a visual deficit in specific reading disability. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 225-267.
  284. Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O. (1988). Effects of an Extensive Program for Stimulating Phonological Awareness in Preschool Children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263-284.
  285. Lyon, R. F. (1982). A Computational Model of Filtering, Detection, and Compression in the Cochlea. Paper presented at the IEEE-ICASSP-82.
  286. Magnan, A., Ecalle, J., & Veuillet, E. (2005). Habiletés phonologiques, identification de mots écrits et déficits auditifs perceptifs chez les enfants dyslexiques : effet d’un entraînement audio-visuel. Revue française de pédagogie, 152, 29-39.
  287. Maison, S., Durrant, J., Gallineau, C., Micheyl, C., & Collet, L. (2001). Delay and temporal integration in medial olivocochlear bundle activation in humans. Ear and Hearing, 22, 65-74.
  288. Maison, S., Micheyl, C., & Collet, L. (1997). Medial olivocochlear efferent system in humans studied with amplitude-modulated tones. Journal of Neurophysiology, 77, 1759-1768.
  289. Manis, F., McBride-Chang, C., Seidenberg, M., Keating, P., Doi, L., Munson, B., et al. (1997). Are Speech Perception Deficits Associated with Developmental Dyslexia? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 211-235.
  290. Manis, F., Seidenberg, M., Doi, L., McBride-Chang, C., & Petersen, A. (1996). On the bases of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157-195.
  291. Marino, C., Giorda, R., Lorusso, M., Vanzin, L., Salandi, N., Nobile, M., et al. (2005). A family-based association study does not support DYX1C1 on 15q21.3 as a candidate gene in developmental dyslexia. European Journal of Human Genetics, 13, 491-499.
  292. Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1984). Function and process in spoken word-recognition. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and Performance, X. London: LEA.
  293. Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition, 71, 71-102.
  294. Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Tyler, L. K. (1980). The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition, 8, 1-71.
  295. Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Welsh, A. (1978). Processing interactions and lexical access during word recognition in continuous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 29-63.
  296. Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Zwitserlood, P. (1989). Accessing spoken words: The importance of word onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 576-585.
  297. Martin, F., & Lovegrove, W. J. (1987). Flicker contrast sensitivity in normal and specifically disabled readers. Perception, 16, 215-221.
  298. Martin, J. G., & Bunnell, H. T. (1981). Perception of anticipatory coarticulation effects. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 69, 559-567.
  299. Massaro, D. W. (1975). Perceptual images, processing time, and perceptual units in speech perception. In D. W. Massaro (Ed.), Understanding Language: An Information-Processing Analysis of Speech Perception, Reading, and Psycholinguistics. New-York: Academic.
  300. Max, L., & Caruso, A. (1997). Acoustic measures of temporal intervals across speaking rates: variability of syllable- and phrase-level relative timing. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1097-1110.
  301. May, B., & McQuone, S. (1995). Effects of bilateral olivocochlear lesions on pure-tone intensity discrimination in cats. Audit. Neurosci., 1, 385-400.
  302. McAnally, K. I., & Stein, J. F. (1996). Auditory temporal coding in dyslexia. Proceedings: Biological Sciences, 263, 961-965.
  303. McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1-86.
  304. McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic finding. Psychological Review, 88, 375-407.
  305. McDougall, S., & Ellis, A. W. (1994). On the problems of comparing dyslexics with other groups of readers (and the quest for the cognitive basis of dyslexia). Early Childhood Development and Care, 101, 33-49.
  306. Mehler, J., Dommergues, J. Y., Frauenfelder, U. H., & Segui, J. (1981). The syllable’s role in speech segmentation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 298-305.
  307. Meng, H., Hager, K., Held, M., Page, G., Olson, R., Pennington, B., et al. (2005). TDT-association analysis of EKN1 and dyslexia in a Colorado twin cohort. Human Genetics, 118, 87-90.
  308. Meng, H., Smith, S., Hager, K., Held, M., Liu, J., Olson, R., et al. (2005). DCDC2 is associated with reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 102, 17053–17058.
  309. Merigan, W., & Maunsell, H. (1993). How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 369-402.
  310. Merzenich, M. M., Jenkins, W. M., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S. L., & Tallal, P. (1996). Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science, 271, 77-81.
  311. Meunier, F., Cenier, T., Barkat, M., & Magrin-Chagnolleau, I. (2002). Mesure d’intelligibilité de segments de parole à l’envers en français.Paper presented at the Journées d'Etude de la Parole, Nancy, France.
  312. Micheyl, C., & Collet, L. (1996). Involvement of the olivocochlear bundle in the detection of tones in noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99, 1604-1610.
  313. Micheyl, C., Khalfa, S., Perrot, X., & Collet, L. (1997). Difference in cochlear efferent activity between musicians and non-musicians. NeuroReport, 8, 1047-1050.
  314. Micheyl, C., Morlet, T., Giraud, A. L., Collet, L., & Morgon, A. (1995). Contralateral suppression of evoked otoacoustic emissions and detection of a multi-tone complex in noise. Acta Otolaryngologica, 115, 178-182.
  315. Micheyl, C., Perrot, X., & Collet, L. (1997). Relationship between auditory intensity discrimination in noise and olivocochlear efferent system activity in humans. Behavioural Neuroscience, 111, 801-807.
  316. Miles, T. R. (1993). Dyslexia: the Pattern of Difficulties. London: Whurr.
  317. Miles, T. R., Haslum, M. N., & Wheeler, T. J. (1998). Gender ratio in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 27-55.
  318. Miller, G. A., Heise, G. A., & Lichten, W. (1951). The intelligibility of speech as a function of the context of the test materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 329-335.
  319. Miller, G. A., & Licklider, J. C. R. (1950). The intelligibility of interrupted speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 22, 167-173.
  320. Miller, G. A., & Nicely, P. E. (1955). An analysis of perceptual confusions among some English consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27, 338-352.
  321. Miller, J. L. (2001). Mapping from acoustic signal to phonetic category: internal category structure, context effects and speeded categorisation. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 683-690.
  322. Miller, J. L., Connine, C. M., Schermer, T. M., & Kluender, K. R. (1983). A possible auditory basis for internal structure of phonetic categories. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73, 2124-2133.
  323. Miller, J. L., & Eimas, P. D. (1995). Speech perception: From signal to word. Annual Review of Psychology 46, 467-492.
  324. Miller, J. L., Green, K. P., & Reeves, A. (1986). Speaking rate and segments : A look at the relation between speech production and speech perception for the voicing contrast. Phonetica, 43, 106-115.
  325. Miller, J. L., Grosjean, F., & Lomanto, C. (1984). Articulation rate and its variability in spontaneous speech: A reanalysis and some implications. Phonetica, 41, 215-225.
  326. Miller, J. L., & Liberman, A. M. (1979). Some effects of later-occuring information on the perception of stop consonant and semivowel. Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 457-465.
  327. Miller, J. L., O'Rourke, T. B., & Volaitis, L. E. (1997). Internal structure of phonetic categories: effects of speaking rate. Phonetica, 54, 121-137.
  328. Miller, J. L., & Volaitis, L. E. (1989). Effect of speaking rate on the perceptual structure of a phonetic category. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 505-512.
  329. Mody, M. (1993). Bases of reading impairment in speech perception: Adeficit in rate of auditory processing or in phonological coding? Unpublished Speech and Hearing Sciences, University of New York.
  330. Mody, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Brady, S. (1997). Speech perception deficits in poor readers: Auditory processing or phonological coding? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 199-231.
  331. Moore, B. C. J. (2003). An introduction to the psychology of hearing (5th ed). San Diego: Academic Press.
  332. Moore, J., Simmons, D., & Guan, Y. (1999). The human olivocochlear system: organization and development. Audiology and Neuro-Otology, 4, 311-325.
  333. Morgan, W. P. (1896). A case of congenital word blindness. British Medical Journal, 2, 1378.
  334. Morlet, T., Goforth, L., Hood, L., Ferber, C., Duclaux, R., & Berlin, C. (1999). Development of human cochlear active mechanism asymmetry: involvement of the medial olivocochlear system? Hearing Research, 134, 153-162.
  335. Morris, D. W., Robinson, L., Turic, D., Duke, M., Webb, V., Milham, C., et al. (2000). Family-based association mapping provides evidence for a gene for reading disability on chromosome 15q. Human Molecular Genetics, 9, 843–848.
  336. Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165-178.
  337. Moulin, A., Collet, L., Veuillet, E., & Morgon, A. (1993). Interrelations between transiently evoked otoacoustic emissions, spontaneous otoacoustic emissions and acoustic distortion products in normally hearing subjects. Hearing Research, 65, 216-233.
  338. Muchnik, C., Rotha, D. A.-E., Othman-Jebara, R., Putter-Katz, H., Shabtai, E. L., & Hildesheimer, M. (2004). Reduced medial olivocochlear bundle system function in children with auditory processing disorders. Audiology and Neuro-Otology, 9, 107-114.
  339. Näätänen, R. (2001). The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). Psychophysiology, 38, 1-21.
  340. Näätänen, R., Lehtokoski, A., Lennes, M., Cheour, M., Huotilainen, M., Livonen, A., et al. (1997). Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses. Nature, 385, 432-434.
  341. Nagarajan, S. S., Mahncke, H., Salz, T., Tallal, P., Roberts, T., & Merzenich, M. M. (1999). Cortical auditory signal processing in poor readers. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 96, 6483–6488.
  342. Newcombe, F., & Ratcliff, G. (1973). Handedness, speech lateralization and ability. Neuropsychologia, 11, 399-407.
  343. Nguyen, N., Wauquier-Gravelines, S., & Tuller, B. (à paraître). The dynamical approach to speech perception: From fine phonetic detail to abstract phoinological categories, (eds.), Mouton [in press]. In I. Chitoran, C. Coupé, E. Marsico & F. Pellegrino (Eds.), Approaches to Phonological Complexity. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  344. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1990). Automaticity: a new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition, 35, 159–182.
  345. Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., Berry, E. L., Jenkins, I. H., Dean, P., & Brooks, D. J. (1999). Association of abnormal cerebellar activation with motor learning difficulties in dyslexic adults. The Lancet, 353, 1662-1667.
  346. Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 508–511.
  347. Niogi, S., & McCandliss, B. (2006). Left lateralized white matter microstructure accounts for individual differences in reading ability and disability. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2178-2188.
  348. Nittrouer, S. (1992). Age-related differences in perceptual effects of formant transitions within syllables and across syllable boundaries. Journal of Phonetics, 20, 351-382.
  349. Nittrouer, S. (1999). Do temporal processing deficits cause phonological processing problems? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 925-942.
  350. Niyogi, P., & Ramesh, P. (2003). The voicing feature for stop consonants: Recognition experiments with continuously spoken alphabets. Speech Communication, 41, 349-367.
  351. Nooteboom, S. G. (1979). Perceptual adjustment to speech rate: a case of backward perceptual normalisation. In Anniversaries in Phonetics: Studia gratulatoria dedicated to Hendrik Mol (pp. 255-269). Amsterdam: Institute of Phonetic Sciences.
  352. Nooteboom, S. G. (1981). Speech rate and segmental perception or the role of words in phoneme identification. In T. Myers, J. Laver & J. Anderson (Eds.), The cognitive representation of speech (pp. 143-150). North Holland.
  353. Nopola-Hemmi, J., Myllyluoma, B., Haltia, T., Taipale, M., Ollikainen, V., Ahonen, T., et al. (2001). A dominant gene for developmental dyslexia on chromosome 3. Journal of Medical Genetics, 38, 658–664.
  354. Norris, D. (1994). Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52, 189-234.
  355. Ohala, J. J. (1983). The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract contraints. In P. F. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech (pp. 189-216). New York: Springer.
  356. Ohala, J. J. (1990). The phonetics and phonology of aspects of assimilation. In J. Kingston & M. E. Beckman (Eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology I: between the grammar and physics of speech (pp. 258-275). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  357. Ohala, J. J. (1996). Speech perception is hearing sounds, not tongues. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99, 1718-1725.
  358. Ohde, R. N. (1979). Acoustic characteristics of stops in CV syllables. Paper presented at the convention of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Atlanta, Georgia.
  359. Ohde, R. N., & Stevens, K. N. (1983). Effect of burst amplitude on the perception of stop consonant place of articulation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 74, 706-714.
  360. Öhman, S. E. G. (1966). Coarticulation in VCV utterances: Spectrographic measurements. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 39, 151-168.
  361. Oldfield, R. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113.
  362. Orton, S. T. (1925). Word-blindness in school children. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 14, 582-615.
  363. Orton, S. T. (1937). Reading, Writing and Speech problems in children. New-York: Norton.
  364. Pallier, C., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Dupoux, E., Christophe, A., & Mehler, J. (1998). Perceptual adjustment to time-compressed speech: A cross-linguistic study. Memory & Cognition, 26, 844-851.
  365. Paracchini, S., Thomas, A., Castro, S., Lai, C., Paramasivam, M., Wang, Y., et al. (2006). The chromosome 6p22 haplotype associated with dyslexia reduces the expression of KIAA0319, a novel gene involved in neuronal migration. Human Molecular Genetics, 15, 1659–1666.
  366. Paulesu, E., Demonet, J. F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., N., B., et al. (2001). Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291, 2165–2167.
  367. Paulesu, E., Frith, U., Snowling, M., Gallagher, A., Morton, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (1996). Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from PET scanning. Brain, 119, 143–157.
  368. Penner, M., & Zhang, T. (1997). Prevalence of spontaneous otoacoustic emissions in adults revisited. Hearing Research, 103, 28-34.
  369. Pennington, B., & Gilger, J. (1996). How is dyslexia transmitted? In C. Chase, G. Rosen & G. Sherman (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: neural, cognitive, and genetic mechanisms (pp. 41-61). Baltimore: York Press.
  370. Pennington, B., van Orden, G., Smith, S., Green, P., & Haith, M. (1990). Phonological Processing Skills and Deficits in Adult Dyslexics. Child Development, 61, 1753-1778.
  371. Perkell, J., & Klatt, D. (1986). Invariance and variability in speech processes: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  372. Perrot, X. (2003). Cortex auditif et système efférent olivocochléaire médian : influence d'une stimulation électrique corticale sur les otoémissions acoustiques et leur atténuation controlatérale. Unpublished Thèse de Doctorat en Médecine, Faculté de Médecine Lyon - Nord, Université Claude Bernard - Lyon 1, Lyon.
  373. Perrot, X., Micheyl, C., Khalfa, S., & Collet, L. (1999). Stronger bilateral efferent influences on cochlear biomechanical acitivity in musicians than in non-musicians. Neuroscience Letters, 262, 167-170.
  374. Perrot, X., Ryvlin, P., Isnard, J., Guénot, M., Catenoix, H., Fischer, C., et al. (2006). Evidence of corticofugal modulation of peripheral auditory activity in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 941-948.
  375. Peterson, G., & Lehiste, I. (1960). Duration of syllable nuclei in english. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 32, 693-703.
  376. Pickett, J. (1999). The Acoustics of Speech Communication: Fundamentals, Speech Perception Theory, and Thechnology. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
  377. Pols, L. C. W. (1983). Three-mode principal component analysis of confusion matrices, based on the identification of Dutch consonants, under various conditions of noise and reverberation. Speech Communication, 2, 275-293.
  378. Port, R. F. (1981). Linguistic timing factors in combination. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 69, 262-274.
  379. Posner, M. (1995). Attention in cognitive neuroscience: an overview In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge: Bradford books
  380. MIT Press.
  381. Price, C. J. (2000). The anatomy of language: Contributions from functional neuroimaging. Journal of Anatomy, 197, 335-359.
  382. Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2003). A further look at the “language-as-fixed-effect fallacy’. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 141-151.
  383. Rack, J. (1994). Dyslexia: the phonological deficit hypothesis. In A. J. Fawcett & R. I. Nicolson (Eds.), Dyslexia in Children: multi-disciplinary perspectives (pp. 5-37). New-York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
  384. Rack, J. (1997). Issues in the assessment of developmental dyslexia in adults: theoretical and applied perspectives. Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 66-76.
  385. Rack, J., Snowling, M. S., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The non-word reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 28-53.
  386. Rajan, R. (2000). Centrifugal Pathways Protect Hearing Sensitivity at the Cochlea in Noisy Environments That Exacerbate the Damage Induced by Loud Sound. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 6684-6693.
  387. Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212–218.
  388. Ramus, F. (2006). A neurological model of dyslexia and other domain-specific developmental disorders with an associated sensorimotor syndrome. In G. D. Rosen (Ed.), The dyslexic brain: New pathways in neuroscience discovery (pp. 75-101). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  389. Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S., Day, B., Castellote, J., White, S., et al. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865.
  390. Raven, J. C. (1938). Progressive matrices: A perceptual test of intelligence. London: H.K. Lewis.
  391. Reed, M. (1989). Speech perception and the discrimination of brief auditory cues in reading disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 270-292.
  392. Remez, R. E., Rubin, P. E., Nygaard, L. C., & Howell, W. A. (1987). Perceptual normalization of vowels produced by sinusoidal voices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 40-61.
  393. Repp, B. H. (1978). Perceptual integration and differenciation of spectral cues for intervocalic stop consonants. Perception & Psychophysics, 24, 471-485.
  394. Repp, B. H. (1980). Accessing phonetic information during perceptual integration of temporally distributed cues. Journal of Phonetics, 8, 185-194.
  395. Repp, B. H. (1984). Categorical perception: issues, methods, findings. In N. Lass (Ed.), Speech and language: advances in basic research and practice (Vol. 10, pp. 243-335).
  396. Repp, B. H., & Liberman, A. M. (1987). Phonetic category boundaries are flexible. In S. Harnad (Ed.), Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition (pp. 89-112). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  397. Rey, V., De Martino, S., Espesser, R., & Habib, M. (2002). Temporal processing and phonological impairment in dyslexia: Effect of phoneme lengthening on order judgment of two consonants. Brain and Language, 80, 576-591.
  398. Rosen, G. D., Bai, J., Wang, Y., Fiondella, C. G., Threlkeld, S. W., LoTurco, J. J., et al. (2007). Disruption of neuronal migration by RNAi of DYX1C1 results in neocortical and hippocampal malformations. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2562-2572.
  399. Rosen, G. D., Burstein, D., & Galaburda, A. M. (2000). Changes in efferent and afferent connectivity in rats with induced cerebrocortical microgyria. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 418, 423-440.
  400. Rosen, S. (1992). Temporal information in speech: Acoustic, auditory and linguistic aspects. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 336, 367-373.
  401. Rosen, S. (2003). Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: is there a deficit? What is its nature? Does it explain anything? Journal of Phonetics, 31, 509-527.
  402. Rosen, S., & Manganari, E. (2001). Is there a relationship between speech and nonspeech auditory processing in children with dyslexia? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 720-736.
  403. Rosenzweig, M. R. (1951). Representations of the two ears at the auditory cortex. American Journal of Physiology, 167, 147-158.
  404. Ruel, J., Nouvian, R., Gervais d'Aldin, C., Pujol, R., Eybalin, M., & Puel, J.-L. (2001). Dopamine inhibition of auditory nerve activity in the adult mammalian cochlea. European Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 977-986.
  405. Rumsey, J., Casanova, M., Mannheim, G., Patronas, N., DeVaughn, N., Hamburger, S., et al. (1996). Corpus callosum morphology, as measured with MRI, in dyslexic men. Biological Psychiatry, 39, 769-775.
  406. Rumsey, J., Donohue, B., Brady, D., Nace, K., Giedd, J., & Andreason, P. (1997). A magnetic resonance imaging study of planum temporale asymmetry in men with developmental dyslexia. Archives of Neurology, 54, 1481-1489.
  407. Rumsey, J., Horwitz, B., Donohue, B., Nace, K., Maisog, J., & Andreason, P. (1999). A functionnal lesion in developmental dyslexia: left angular gyral blood flow predicts severity. Brain and Language, 70, 187-204.
  408. Russo, N. M., Nicol, T. G., Zecker, S. G., Hayes, E. A., & Kraus, N. (2005). Auditory training improves neural timing in the human brainstem. Behavioural Brain Research, 156, 95-103.
  409. Rutter, M., Caspi, A., Fergusson, D., Horwood, L., Goodman, R., Maughan, B., et al. (2004). Sex differences in developmental reading disability—new findings from 4 epidemiological studies. Journal of the American Medical Association, 291, 2007–2012.
  410. Saberi, K., & Perrott, D. R. (1999). Cognitive restoration of reversed speech. Nature, 398, 760.
  411. Saerens, M., Serniclaes, W., & Beeckmans, R. (1989). Acoustic versus contextual factors in stop voicing perception in spontaneous French. Language and speech, 32, 291-314.
  412. Samuel, A. G. (1981). Phonemic restoration: insights from a new methodology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 474-494.
  413. Scarborough, H. (1984). Continuity between childhood dyslexia and adult reading. Brain Journal of Psychology, 75, 329-348.
  414. Scerri, T. S., Fisher, S. E., Francks, C., MacPhie, I. L., Paracchini, S., Richardson, A. J., et al. (2004). Putative functional alleles of DYX1C1 are not associated with dyslexia susceptibility in a large sample of sibling pairs from the UK. Journal of Medical Genetics, 41, 853-857.
  415. Schönwiesner, M., Krumbholz, K., Rübsamen, R., Fink, G., & von Cramon, D. (2007). Hemispheric Asymmetry for Auditory Processing in the Human Auditory Brain Stem, Thalamus, and Cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 492-499.
  416. Schulte-Körne, G., Bartling, J., Deimel, W., & Remschmidt, H. (2004). Motion-onset VEPs in dyslexia. Evidence for visual perceptual deficit. Neuroreport, 15, 1075–1078.
  417. Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J., & Remschmidt, H. (1998). Auditory processing and dyslexia: Evidence for a specific speech processing deficit. NeuroReport, 9, 337-340.
  418. Schulte-Körne, G., Grimm, T., Nothen, M., Müller-Myhsok, B., Cichon, S., Vogt, I., et al. (1998). Evidence for linkage of spelling disability to chromosome 15. American Journal of Human Genetics, 63, 279–282.
  419. Schumacher, J., Hoffmann, P., Schmäl, C., Schulte-Körne, G., & Nöthen, M. (2007). Genetics of dyslexia: the evolving landscape. Journal of Medical Genetics, 44, 289-297.
  420. Schwartz, J., & Tallal, P. (1980). Rate of acoustic change may underlie hemispheric specialization for speech perception. Science, 207, 1380-1381.
  421. Scott, S. K., & Johnsrude, I. S. (2003). The neuroanatomical and functional organization of speech perception. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 100-107.
  422. Scott, S. K., & Wise, R. J. S. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of prelexical processing in speech perception. Cognition, 92, 13-45.
  423. Sebastián-Gallés, N., Dupoux, E., Segui, J., & Mehler, J. (1992). Contrasting syllabic effects in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 18-32.
  424. Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523-568.
  425. Seidenberg, M. S., Plaut, D. C., Petersen, A. S., McClelland, J. L., & McRae, K. (1994). Nonword pronunciation and models of word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1177-1196.
  426. Serniclaes, W. (1987). Étude expérimentale de la perception du trait de voisement des occlusives du français. Université Libre de Bruxelles, Bruxelles, Belgique.
  427. Serniclaes, W., Bogliotti, C., & Carré, R. (2003). Perception of consonant place of articulation: phonological categories meet natural boundaries. Paper presented at the The 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona, Spain.
  428. Serniclaes, W., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2003). Categorical perception of speech sounds and dyslexia. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition, 10.
  429. Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Carré, R., & Demonet, J. F. (2001). Perceptual categorization of speech sounds in dyslexics. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 384-399.
  430. Serniclaes, W., Van Heghe, S., Mousty, P., Carré, R., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2004). Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 336-361.
  431. Seymour, P. H. K. (1990). Developmental dyslexia. In M. W. Eysenck (Ed.), Cognitive psychology: An international review (pp. 135-196). Chichester: Wiley.
  432. Shadle, C. H. (1997). The Aerodynamics of Speech. In W. J. Hardcastle & J. Laver (Eds.), Handbook of Phonetics (pp. 33-64): Blackwell's.
  433. Shannon, R. V., Zeng, F.-G., Kamath, V., Wygonski, J., & Ekelid, M. (1995). Speech recognition with primarily temporal cues. Science, 270, 303-304.
  434. Shapleske, J., Rossell, S., Woodruff, P., & David, A. (1999). The planum temporale: a systematic, quantitative review of its structural, functional and clinical significance. Brain Research Reviews, 29, 26-49.
  435. Shaywitz, B., Shaywitz, S., Blachman, B., Pugh, K., Fulbright, R., Skudlarski, P., et al. (2004). Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically-based intervention. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 926-933.
  436. Shaywitz, S. (1998). Dyslexia. New England Journal of Medicine(338), 307-312.
  437. Shaywitz, S., Fletcher, J., Holahan, J., Shneider, A., Marchione, K., Stuebing, K., et al. (1999). Persistence of dyslexia: the Connecticut Longitudinal Study at adolescence. Pediatrics, 104, 1351–1359.
  438. Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Fletcher, J., & Escobar, M. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. Results of the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, 998-1002.
  439. Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Fulbright, R., Skudlarski, P., Mencl, W., Constable, R., et al. (2003). Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult outcome of childhood reading disability. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 25–33.
  440. Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Pugh, K., Fulbright, R., Constable, R., Mencl, W., et al. (1998). Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 95, 2636–2641.
  441. Shinn, P., Blumstein, S. E., & Jongman, A. (1985). Limitations of context conditioned effects in the perception of [b] and [w]. Perception & Psychophysics, 38, 397-407.
  442. Shtyrov, Y., Kujala, T., Ahveninen, J., Tervaniemi, M., Alku, P., Ilmoniemi, R. J., et al. (1998). Background acoustic noise and the hemispheric lateralization of speech processing in the human brain: magnetic mismatch negativity study. Neuroscience Letters, 251, 141-144.
  443. Silani, G., Frith, U., Demonet, J. F., Fazio, F., Perani, D., Price, C. J., et al. (2005). Brain abnormalities underlying altered activation in dyslexia: a voxel based morphometry study. Brain, 128, 2453-2461.
  444. Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Bergman, E., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2000). Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: A magnetic source imaging approach. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 809-816.
  445. Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Bergman, E., & Fishbeck, K. (2000). Brain activation profiles in dyslexic children during non-word reading: a magnetic source imaging study. Neuroscience Letters, 290, 61–65.
  446. Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Bergman, E., Breier, J. I., Foorman, B. R., Castillo, E. M., et al. (2002). Dyslexia-specific brain activation profile becomes normal following successful remedial training. Neurology, 58, 1203-1213.
  447. Skoff, B. F., Mersky, A. F., & Turner, D. (1980). Prolonged brainstem transmission time in autism. Psychiatry Research, 2, 157-166.
  448. Smith, S. D., Kimberling, W. J., Pennington, B. F., & Lubs, H. A. (1983). Specific reading disability: identification of an inherited form through linkage analysis. Science, 219, 1345–1347.
  449. Smits, R., Ten Bosch, L., & Collier, R. (1996a). Evaluation of various sets of acoustic cues for the perception of prevocalic stop consonants. I. Perception experiment. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100, 3852-3864.
  450. Smits, R., ten Bosch, L., & Collier, R. (1996b). Evaluation of various sets of acoustic cues for the perception of prevocalic stop consonants. II. Modeling and evaluation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100, 3865-3881.
  451. Snoeren, N. D., Hallé, P. A., & Segui, J. (2006). A voice for the voiceless: Production and perception of assimilated stops in French. Journal of Phonetics, 34, 241-268.
  452. Snowling, M., Goulandris, N., Bowlby, M., & Howell, P. (1986). Segmentation and speech perception in relation to reading skill: A developmental analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 489-507.
  453. Snowling, M. J. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Research, 43, 219-234.
  454. Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford: Blackwell.
  455. Snowling, M. J. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7, 37-46.
  456. Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Stothard, S. E. (2000). Is pre-school language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 587-600.
  457. Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Casalis, S. (1996). Lire. Lecture/écriture: Acquisition et troubles du développement. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
  458. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Lacert, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2000). On subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from processing time and accuracy scores. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 88-104.
  459. Stein, J. F. (1994). Developmental dyslexia, neural timing and hemispheric lateralisation. International Journal of Psychophysiology 18, 241-249.
  460. Stein, J. F. (2001). The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7, 12-36.
  461. Stein, J. F., & Fowler, S. (1981). Visual dyslexia. Trends in Neurosciences, 4, 77-80.
  462. Stein, J. F., Richardson, A. J., & Fowler, M. S. (2000). Monocular occlusion can improve binocular control and reading in dyslexics. Brain, 123, 164-170.
  463. Stein, J. F., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 147–152.
  464. Stephan, K. E., Marshall, J. C., Friston, K. J., Rowe, J. B., Ritzl, A., Zilles, K., et al. (2003). Lateralized cognitive processes and lateralized task control in the human brain. Science, 301, 384-386.
  465. Stevens, K. N., & Blumstein, S. E. (1978). Invariant cues for place of articulation in stop consonants. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 64, 1358-1368.
  466. Stevens, K. N., & Blumstein, S. E. (1981). The search for invariant acoustic correlates of phonetic features. In P. D. Eimas & J. L. Miller (Eds.), Perspectives on the study of speech (pp. 1-38). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  467. Stevens, K. N., Keyser, S. J., & Kawasaki, H. (1986). Toward a phonetic and phonological theory of redundant features. In J. S. Perkell & D. H. Klatt (Eds.), Invariance and variability in speech processes (pp. 426-449). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  468. Strange, W. (1989). Dynamic specification of coarticulated vowels spoken in sentence context. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 85, 2135-2153.
  469. Strange, W., Jenkins, J. J., & Johnson, T. L. (1983). Dynamic specification of coarticulated vowels. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 74, 695-705.
  470. Streeter, L. A., & Nigro, G. N. (1979). The role of medial consonant transitions in word perception. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 65, 1533-1541.
  471. Studdert-Kennedy, M. (2002). Mirror neurons, vocal imitation and the evoluation of particulate speech. In M. Stamenov & V. Gallese (Eds.), Mirror neurons and the evolution of brain and language (pp. 207-227). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
  472. Suga, N., Xiao, Z., Ma, X., & Ji, W. (2002). Plasticity and Corticofugal Modulation for Hearing in Adult Animals. Neuron, 36, 9-18.
  473. Summerfield, Q. (1975). Aerodynamics versus mechanics in the control of voicing onset in consonant–vowel syllables. In Speech perception. Belfast: Queen’s University, Department of Psychology.
  474. Summerfield, Q. (1981). Articulatory rate and perceptual constancy in phonetic perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 7, 1074-1095.
  475. Sussman, H. M., McCaffrey, H. A., & Matthews, S. A. (1991). An investigation of locus equations as a source of relational invariance for stop place categorization. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 90, 1309-1325.
  476. Taft, M. (1979). Lexical access via an orthographic code: the Basic Orthographic Syllabic Structure (BOSS). Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 21-39.
  477. Taipale, M., Kaminen, N., Nopola-Hemmi, J., Haltia, T., Myllyluoma, B., Lyytinen, H., et al. (2003). A candidate gene for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein dynamically regulated in brain. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 100, 11553–11558.
  478. Tallal, P. (1980a). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9, 182-198.
  479. Tallal, P. (1980b). Language and reading: Some perceptual prerequisites. Annals of Dyslexia, 30, 170-178.
  480. Tallal, P. (1984). Temporal or Phonetic Processing Deficit in Dyslexia? Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 167-169.
  481. Tallal, P., Allard, L., Miller, S. L., & Curtiss, S. (1997). Academic outcomes of language impaired children. In C. Hulme & M. Snowling (Eds.), Dyslexia: Biology, cognition and intervention. London: Whurr Publishers Limited.
  482. Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S. S., et al. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81-83.
  483. Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973). Developmental aphasia: impaired rate of non-verbal processing as a function of sensory modality. Neuropsychologia, 11, 389-398.
  484. Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia: rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia, 12, 83-93.
  485. Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1975). Developmental aphasia: The perception of brief vowels and extended stop consonants. Neuropsychologia, 13, 69-74.
  486. Tallal, P., & Stark, R. E. (1981). Speech acoustic-cue discrimination abilities of normally developing and language-impaired children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 69, 568-574.
  487. Tartter, V. C., Kat, D., Samuel, A. G., & Repp, B. H. (1983). Perception of intervocalic stop consonants: the contributions of closure duration and formant transitions. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 74, 715-725.
  488. Temple, E., Deutsch, G. K., Poldrack, R. A., Miller, S. L., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., et al. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 100, 2860-2865.
  489. Temple, E., Poldrack, R. A., Protopapas, A., Nagarajan, S. S., Salz, T., Tallal, P., et al. (2000). Disruption of the neural response to rapid acoustic stimuli in dyslexia: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 13907-13912.
  490. Temple, E., Poldrack, R. A., Salidis, J., Deutsch, G. K., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., et al. (2001). Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: an fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12, 299–307.
  491. Thompson, A. M., & Schofield, B. R. (2000). Afferent projections of the superior olovary complex. Microscopy Research and Technique, 51, 330-354.
  492. Treiman, R. (1985). Spelling of stop consonants after /s/ by children and adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 261-282.
  493. Trubetzkoy, N. S. (1939). Grundzüge der Phonologie. Travaux du cercle linguistique de Prague, 7, 272.
  494. Tuller, B., Case, P., Ding, M., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1994). The nonlinear dynamics of speech categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 3-16.
  495. Turkeltaub, P. E., Gareau, L., Flowers, D. L., Zeffiro, T. A., & Eden, G. F. (2003). Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 767-773.
  496. Valdois, S., Bosse, M.-L., Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., Zorman, M., David, D., et al. (2003). Phonological and visual processing deficits can dissociate in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from two case studies. Reading and Writing, 16, 541-572.
  497. Valdois, S., Bosse, M.-L., & Tainturier, M.-J. (2004). The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia, 10, 339-363.
  498. Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 15, 181-198.
  499. van Wieringen, A., & Pols, L. C. W. (1995). Discrimination of single and complex consonant-vowel-and vowel-consonant-like formant transitions. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98, 1304-1312.
  500. Velenovsky, D., & Glattke, T. (2002). The effect of noise bandwidth on the contralateral suppression of transient evoked otoacoustic emissions. Hearing Research, 164, 39-48.
  501. Vellutino, E. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  502. Verbrugge, R. R., & Isenberg, D. (1978). Syllable timing and vowel perception. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63, S4.
  503. Veuillet, E., Bazin, F., & Collet, L. (1999). Objective evidence of peripheral auditory disorders in learning-impaired children. Journal of Audiological Medicine, 8, 18-29.
  504. Veuillet, E., Collet, L., & Duclaux, R. (1991). Effect of contralateral acoustic stimulation on active cochlear micromechanical properties in human subjects: Dependence on stimulus variables. Journal of Neurophysiology, 65, 724-734.
  505. Veuillet, E., Georgieff, N., Philibert, B., Dalery, J., Marie-Cardine, M., & Collet, L. (2001). Abnormal peripheral auditory asymmetry in schizophrenia. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 70, 88-94.
  506. Veuillet, E., Khalfa, S., & Collet, L. (1999). Clinical relevance of medial efferent auditory pathways. Scandinavian Audiology. Supplementum, 51, 53-62.
  507. Veuillet, E., Magnan, A., & Ecalle, J. (2004). Déficits auditifs perceptifs et capacités en lecture chez les enfants dyslexiques : effet d'un entraînement audio-visuel. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 14, 103-132.
  508. Veuillet, E., Magnan, A., Ecalle, J., Thai-Van, H., & Collet, L. (2007). Auditory processing disorder in children with reading disabilities: Effect of audiovisual training. Brain, 130, 2915-2928.
  509. Vidyasagar, T. R. (2004). Neural underpinnings of dyslexia as a disorder of visuo-spatial attention. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 87, 4-10.
  510. Vigneau, M., Beaucousin, V., Hervé, P. Y., Duffau, H., Crivello, F., Houdé, O., et al. (2006). Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage, 30, 1414-1432.
  511. Vinckenbosch, E., Robichon, F., & Eliez, S. (2005). Gray matter alteration in dyslexia: converging evidence from volumetric and voxel-by-voxel MRI analyses. Neuropsychologia, 43, 324-331.
  512. Virole, B., & Cosnier, J. (2004). La perception phonétique. In Psychologie de la surdité (pp. 101-133). Bruxelles: De Boeck Université.
  513. Wagner, R., & Torgesen, J. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212.
  514. Wang, Y., Paramasivam, M., Thomas, A., Bai, J., Kaminen-Ahola, N., Kere, J., et al. (2006). DYX1C1 functions in neuronal migration in developing neocortex. Neuroscience, 143, 515-522.
  515. Warr, W., & Guinan, J. J. (1979). Efferent innervation of the organ of corti: two separate systems. Brain Research, 173, 152-155.
  516. Warren, R. M. (1970). Perceptual restoration of missing speech sounds. Science, 167, 392-393.
  517. Warren, R. M. (1974). Auditory temporal discrimination by trained listeners. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 237-256.
  518. Wernicke, C. (1874). Der Aphasische Symptomencomplex Breslau: Cohn and Weigert.
  519. Whalen, D. H. (1991). Subcategorical phonetic mismatches and lexical access. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 351-360.
  520. Wible, B., Nicol, T., & Kraus, N. (2005). Correlation between brainstem and cortical auditory processes in normal and language-impaired children. Brain, 128, 417-423.
  521. Wigg, K., Couto, J., Feng, Y., Anderson, B., Cate-Carter, T., Macciardi, F., et al. (2004). Support for EKN1 as the susceptibility locus for dyslexia on 15q21. Molecular Psychiatry, 9, 1111–1121.
  522. Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (1998). Poor Reading: A Deficit in Skill-Automatization or a Phonological Deficit? Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 321-340.
  523. Witton, C., Talcott, J. B., Hansen, P. C., Richardson, A. J., Griffiths, T. D., Rees, A., et al. (1998). Sensitivity to dynamic auditory and visual stimuli predicts nonword reading ability in both dyslexic and normal readers. Current Biology, 8, 791-797.
  524. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.
  525. Wright, B. A., Lombardino, L. J., King, W. M., Puranik, C. S., Leonard, C. M., & Merzenich, M. M. (1997). Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language-impaired children. Nature, 387, 176-178.
  526. Wright, R. (2001). Perceptual cues in contrast maintenance. In E. Hume & K. Johnson (Eds.), The role of speech perception in phonology (pp. 251-277). San Diego: Academic Press.
  527. Xiao, Z., & Suga, N. (2002). Reorganization of the cochleotopic map in the bat’s auditory system by inhibition. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 99, 15743-15748.
  528. Zagar, D., Jourdain, C., & Lété, B. (1995). Le diagnostic cognitif des capacités de lecture : le logiciel ECCLA : (Evaluation diagnostic des Capacités Cognitives du Lecteur Adulte). Revue française de pédagogie, 113, 19-29.
  529. Zatorre, R. J., Belin, P., & Penhune, V. B. (2002). Structure and function of auditory cortex: Music and speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 37-46.
  530. Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., Meyer, E., & Gjedde, A. (1992). Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science, 256, 846-849.
  531. Zemlin, W. R., Daniloff, R. G., & Shriner, T. H. (1968). The difficulty of listening to time-compressed speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 11, 875-881.
  532. Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., Alario, F.-X., & Lorenzi, C. (2005). Deficits in speech perception predict language learning impairment. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences of the united states of america, 102, 14110–14115.
  533. Ziegler, J. C., Van Orden, G. C., & Jacobs, A. M. (1997). Phonology can help or hurt the perception of print. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 845-860.
  534. Zue, V. (1976). Acoustic Characteristics of Stop Consonants: A Controlled Study.M.I.T.