Références

  1. Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261-295.
  2. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence (Ed.), The psychology if learning and motivation: Advances in Research and theory 2 ( pp. 89-195). New York, Academic Press.
  3. Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.
  4. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1977). Recency re-examined. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and Performance, 6 (pp. 647-667). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  5. Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and brain sciences, 22, 577-660.
  6. Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 513-562.
  7. Barsalou, L.W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645.
  8. Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 84-91.
  9. Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269, 1115-1118.
  10. Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). Failure to respond automatically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 215-225.
  11. Bensafi, M., Sobel, N., & Khan, R. M. (2007). Hedonic-specific activity in piriform cortex during odor imagery mimics that during odor perception. Neurophysiology, 98(6), 3254-3262.
  12. Blake, R., Sobel, V. K., & James, T. W. (2004). Neural synergy between kinetic vision and touch. Psychological Science, 15, 397-402.
  13. Blaxton, T. A. (1989). Investigating dissociations among memory measures: Support for a transfer-appropriate processing framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 657-668.
  14. Borghi, A. M., Bonfiglioli, C., Lugli, L., Ricciardelli, P., Rubichi, S., & Nicoletti, R. (2007). Are visual stimuli sufficient to evoke motor information ? Studies with hand primes. Neuroscience Letters, 411, 17-21.
  15. Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., & Freund, H. J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner : An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 401-404.
  16. Brunel, L., Vallet, G., Oker, A., & Versace, R. (en préparation). Implicit and explicit emergence of the knowledge: Evidence for a level of integration process.
  17. Calvert, G. A., & Thesen, T. (2004). Multisensory integration: Methodological approaches and emerging principles in the human brain. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 98, 191-205.
  18. Cohen, J., McWhinney, B., Flatt, M. & Provost J. (1993). PsyScope designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers.  Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 25, 257-271.
  19. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407-428.
  20. Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240-247.
  21. Craik, F. I., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684.
  22. Craik, F. I., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268-294.
  23. Damas, L., Versace, R. & Mille, A. (2001). Etudier la mémoire humaine pour concevoir des assistants informatiques à l’apprentissage. Actes de RàPC’ 2001, (PP. 11-16). Plateforme AFIA. Grenoble, Juin.
  24. Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation : A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25-62.
  25. Damasio, A. R. (1995). L'erreur de Descartes. Paris : Odile Jacob.
  26. Decety, J., & Boisson, D. (1990). Effect of brain and spinal cord injuries on motor imagery. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 240, 39-43.
  27. Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., Germain, M., & Pastène, J. (1991). Vegetative response during imagined movement is proportional to mental effort. Behavioral Brain Research, 42, 1-5.
  28. Ducreux, A., Marsot-Dupuch, K., Lasjaunias, P., Oppenheim, C., & Fredy, D. (2003). Lyrical and musical auditive mental imagery in functional MRI. Journal of Neuroradiology, 30(1), 18-24.
  29. Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2000). Micro-affordance: The potentiation of components of action by seen objects. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 451-471.
  30. Eustache, F., & Desgranges, B. (2003). Concepts et modèles en neuropsychologie de la mémoire: entre théorie et partique clinique. In T. Meulemans, B. Desgranges, S., Adam, et E., Eustache (Eds.), Evaluation et prise en charge des troubles mnésiques (pp. 13-49). Marseille: Solal.
  31. Falchier, A., Clavagnier, S., Barone, P., & Kennedy, H. (2002). Anatomical evidence of multimodal integration in primate striate cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(13), 5749-5759.
  32. Ferrier, L., Staudt, A., reilhac, G., Jiménez, M., & Brouillet, D. (2007). Influence of typical size of objects in a categorization task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 316-321.
  33. Foxe, J. J., Morocz, I. A., Murray, M. M., Higgins, B. A., Javitt, D. C., & Schroeder, C. E. (2000). Multisensory auditory-somatosensory interactions in early cortical processing revealed by high-density electrical mapping. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 77-83.
  34. Foxe, J., & Schroeder, C. E. (2005). The case for feedforward multisensory convergence during early cortical processing. Neuroreport, 16(5), 419-423.
  35. Frak, V., Paulignan, Y., & Jeannerod, M. (2001). Orientation of the opposition axis in mentally simulated grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 136, 120-127.
  36. Fu, K. M. G., Johnston, T. A., Shah, A. S., Arnold, L., Smiley, J., Hackett, T. A., Garraghty, P. E., & Schroeder, C. E. (2003). Auditory cortical neurons respond to somatosensory stimulation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(20), 7510-7515.
  37. Gallese, V. (2003). The manifold nature of interpersonal relations : The quest for a common mechanism. Philosophical Transactions-Royal Society of London. Biological sciences, 358(1431), 517-528.
  38. Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593-609.
  39. Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. I. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mindreading. Cognitive Sciences, 12(2), 493-501.
  40. Gazzola, V., van der Worp, H., Mulder, T., Wicker, B., Rizzolatti, G., & Keysers, C. (2007). Aplasics born without hands mirror the goal of hand actions with their Feet. Current Biology, 17, 1235-1240.
  41. Gentilucci, M., Benuzzi, F., Bertolani, L., Daprati, E., & Gangitano, M. (2000). Recognizing a hand by grasp. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 125-135.
  42. Gerardin, E., Sirigu, A., Lehericy, S., Poline, J. B., Gaymard, B., Marsault, C., Agid, Y., & Le Bihan, D. (2000). Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 1093-1104.
  43. Ghazanfar, A. A., Maier, J. X., Hoffman, K. L., & Logothetis, N. K. (2005). Multisensory integration of dynamic faces and voices in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(20), 5004-5012.
  44. Giard, M. H., & Peronnet, F. (1999). Auditory-visual integration during multimodal object recognition in humans : A behavioral and electrophysiological study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(5), 473-490.
  45. Gil-da-Costa, R., Braun, A., Lopes, M., Hauser, M. D., Carson, R. E., Herscovitch, P., & Martin, A. (2004). Toward an evolutionary perspective on conceptual representation: Species-specific calls activate visual and affective processing systems in the macaque. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(50), 17516-17521.
  46. Glenberg A. M. (1997). What memory is for? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 1-55.
  47. Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 558-565.
  48. Goldman-Ravic, P. S., Scalaidhe, S. P. O., & Chafee, M. V. (2000). Domain specificity in cognitive systems.In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (pp. 839-847). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  49. Gottfried, J. A., Smith, A. P., Rugg, M. D., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Remembrance of odors past: Human olfactory cortex in cross-modal recognition memory. Neuron, 42. 687 -695.
  50. Grèzes, J., Tucker, M., Armory, J., Ellis, R., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). Objects automatically potentiate action: An fMRi study of implicit processing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2735-2740.
  51. Haris, R., Forss, N., Avikainen, S., Kirveskari, E., Salenius, S., & Rizzolatti, G. (1998). Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation : A neuromagnetic study. Neurobiology, 95, 15061-15065.
  52. Helbig, H. B., Graf, M., & Kiefer, M. (2006). The role of action representations in visual object recognition. Experimental Brain Research, 172(2), 1432-1106.
  53. Hintzman, D. L. (1986). “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 93, 411-428.
  54. Hoffman, J. (1982). Representation of concepts and classification of objects. In F. Klix, J. Hoffmann, & E. Van Dermeer (Eds.), Cognitive Research in Psychology (pp. 72-89). Amsterdam: North Holland Publishing Company.
  55. Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 495-500.
  56. Jacoby, L. L., & Witherspoon, D. (1982). Remembering without awareness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 300-324.
  57. James, T. W., James, K. H., Humphrey, G. K., & Goodale, M. A. (2005). Do visual and tactile object representations share the same neural substrate? In M. A. Heller and S. Ballesteros (Eds.), Touch and blindness : Psychology and neuroscience. Mahwah, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum.
  58. Jones, E. G. (1970). An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. Brain, 93, 793-820.
  59. Kan, I. P., Barsalou, L. W., Solomon, K. O., Minor, J. K., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2003). Role of mental imagery in a property verification task : fMRI evidence for perceptual representations of conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 525-540.
  60. Kaschak, M. P., Madden, C. J., Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., Aveyard, M., Blanchard, A. A., & Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition, 94, 79-89.
  61. Kaschak, M. P., Zwaan, R. A., Aveyard, M., & Yaxley, R. (2006). Perception of auditory motion affects language processing. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 733-744.
  62. Kolers, P. A. (1973). Remembering operations. Memory and Cognition, 12, 347-355.
  63. Kosslyn, S. M., Ball, T. M., & Reiser, B. (1978). Visual images preserve metric spatial information : Evidence from studies of image scanning. Journal of Experimental Psychology :Human perception and performance, 4(1), 47-60.
  64. Labeye,E., Oker, A., Badard, G., & Versace, R. (2008). Activation and integration of motors components in a short-term priming paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 129, 108-111.
  65. Lacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3, 529-535.
  66. Lawrence, A. D., Calder, A. J., McGowan, S. W., & Grasby, P. M. (2002). Selective disruption of the recognition of facial expressions of anger. NeuroReport 13, 881-884
  67. Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492-527.
  68. Lotze, M., Montoya, P., Erb, M., Hülsmann, E., Flor, H., Klose, U., Birbaumer, N., & Grodd, W. (1999). Activation of cortical and cerebella motor areas during executed and imagined hand movements : An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(5), 491-501.
  69. Macaluso, E., Frith, C. D., & Driver, J. (2000). Modulation of human visual cortex by crossmodal spatial attention. Science, 289(5482), 1206-1208.
  70. Martin, A., & Chao, L. L. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: Structure and processes. Neurobiology, 11, 194-201.
  71. Martins, S., Guillery-Girard, B., & Eustache, F. (2006). Modèles de la mémoire humaine: concepts et modèles en neuropsychologie de l’adulte et de l’enfant. Epilepsies, 18, 4-14.
  72. Martin, A., Haxby, J. V., Lalonde, F. M., Wiggs, C. L., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science, 379, 649-652.
  73. Martin, A., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Category specificity and the brain : The sensory/motor model of semantic representations of objects. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 839-847). Cambridge, Massachusetts : MIT Press.
  74. Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649-652.
  75. Massaro, D. W. (1998). Perceiving talking faces: From speech perception to a behavioral principle. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
  76. McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1986). A distributed model of human learning and memory. In J. L., McClelland, D. E., Rumelhart, & the PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel Distributed Processing (pp. 170-215). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  77. Meddin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207-238.
  78. Molholm, S., Ritter, W., Murray, M. M., Javitt, D. C., Schroeder, C. E., & Foxe, J. J. (2002). Multisensory auditory-visual interactions during early sensory processing in humans: A high-density electrical mapping study. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 115-128.
  79. Mondillon, L., Niedenthal, P.M., Vermeulen, N., & Winkielman, P. (Submitted). Embodiment of emotional concepts: Evidence from EMG Measures.
  80. Murray, M. M., Molholm, S., Michel, C. M., Heslenfeld, D. J., Ritter, W., Javitt, D. C., Schroeder, C. E., & Foxe, J. J. (2005). Grabbing your ear: Rapid auditory-somatosensory. multisensory interactions in low-level sensory cortices are not constrained by stimulus alignment. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 963-974.
  81. Myung, J. Y., Blumstein, S. E., & Sedivy, J. C. (2006). Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: Manipulation knowledge of objects. Cognition, 98, 223-243.
  82. Nicolas, S. (2003). Mémoire et conscience. Paris: A. Collin (Coll. Cursus).
  83. Niedenthal, P.M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316, 1002-1005.
  84. Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9(3), 184-211.
  85. Niedenthal, P. M., Brauer, M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Innes-Ker, A. H. (2001). When did her smile drop? Facial mimicry and the influences of emotional state on the detection of change in emotional expression.Cognition & Emotion, 15, 853–864.
  86. Oker, A., & Versace, R. (in press). Spatial Distinctiveness effect in Catégorization. The European Journal of Cognitive Psychology.
  87. Oliver, G. (2006). Visuomotor priming of a manual reaching movement during a perceptual decision task. Brain Research, 1124, 81-85.
  88. Packard, M. G., & Cahill, L. (2001). Affective modulation of multiple memory systems. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 752-756.
  89. Papaxanthis, C., Pozzo, T., Kaspirinski, R., & Berthoz, A. (2003). Comparison of actual and imagined execution of whole-body movements after a long exposure to microgravity. Neuroscience Letters, 339(1), 41-44.
  90. Pecher, D., Zanolie, K., & Zeelenberg, R. (2007). Verifying visual properties in sentence verfication facilitates picture recognition memory. Experimental Psychology, 54(3), 173-179.
  91. Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Verifying different-modality properties for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science, 14(2), 119-124.
  92. Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2004). Sensorimotor simulations underlie conceptual representations : Modality-specific effects of prior activation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 164-167.
  93. Proverbio, A. M., Del Zotto, M., & Zani, A. (2007). The emergence of semantic categorization in early visual processing : ERP indices of animal vs. Artefact recognition. BMC Neuroscience, 8(24), 1-16.
  94. Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M., & Chen, Y. (1996). Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production.Psychological Science, 7, 226–231.
  95. Rockland, K. S., & Ojima, H. (2001). Calcarine area V1 as a multimodal convergence area. Society for Neuroscience Abstract, 27, 511-20.
  96. Rockland, K. S., & Ojima, H. (2003). Multisensory convergence in calcarine visual areas in macaque monkey. Journal Psychophysiology, 50, 19-26.
  97. Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch, & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  98. Rousset, S. (2000). Les conceptions “système unique” de la mémoire: aspects théoriques. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 10(1), 27-51.
  99. Rubinsten, O., & Henik, A. (2002). Is an ant larger than a lion ?. Acta Psychologica , 111, 141-154.
  100. Schroeder, C. E., & Foxe, J. J. (2002). The timing and laminar profile of converging inputs to multisensory areas of the macaque neocortex. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 187-198.
  101. Schroeder, C. E., Lindsley, R. W., Specht, C., Marcovici, A., Smiley, J. F., & Javitt, D. C. (2001). Somatosensory input to auditory association cortex in the macaque monkey. Journal Neurophysiology, 85, 1322-1327.
  102. Schroeder, C. E., Smiiley, J., Fu, K. M. G., McGinnis, T., O’Connell, M. N., & Hackett, T. A. (2003). Anatomical mechanisms and functional implications of multisensory convergence in early cortical processing. Journal Psychophysiology, 50, 5-18.
  103. Schröger, E., & Widmann, A. (1998). Speeded responses to audiovisual signal changes result from bimodal integration. Psychophysiology, 35, 755-759.
  104. Scoville, W. B. & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 20, 11-21.
  105. Setic, M., & Domijan, D. (2007). The influence of vertical spatial orientation on property verification. Language and Cognitive Processes, 2(2), 297-312.
  106. Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. K. (1970). Independent functioning of verbal memory stores. A neuropsychological study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 261-273.
  107. Sheppard, R. N. & Feng, C. (1972). A chronometric study of mental paper folding. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 228-243.
  108. Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2003). The similarity-in-topography principle : Reconciling theories of conceptual deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3-6), 451-486.
  109. Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L. W. (2005). Picture of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1602-1608.
  110. Smith, E. E., Shoben, E., & Rips, L. U. (1974). Structure and process in semantic memory: A feature model for semantic decision. Psychological Review, 81, 214-241.
  111. Solomon, K. O., & Barsalou, L. W. (2001). Representing Properties Locally. Cognitive Psychology, 4(2), 129-169.
  112. Solomon, K. O., & Barsalou, L. W. (2004). Perceptual simulation in property verification. Memory & Cognition, 32, 244-259.
  113. Spivey, M. J., Tyler, M. J., Richardson, D. C., & Young, E. E. (2000). Eye movements during comprehension of spoken scene descriptions. Proceedings of the twenty-second Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 487-492). Erlbaum : Mawhah, NJ.
  114. Stenfield, R., & Zwaan, R. A. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12(2), 153-156.
  115. Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (1999). Action planning and the temporal binding of response Codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25(6), 1625-1640.
  116. Stoet, G., & Hommel, B. (2002). Interaction between feature binding in perception and action. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and Performance, XIX (pp. 538-552). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  117. Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P. (1999). Affectively burnt in: A proposed role of the right frontal lobe. In E. Tulving (Ed.), Memory, consciousness, and the brain : The Tallinn Conference (pp. 215-227). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
  118. Tiberghien, G. (1997). La mémoire oubliée. Liège: Mardaga.
  119. Treisman, A. M. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95, 15-48.
  120. Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136.
  121. Tseng, M. J., & Bergen, B. K. (2005). Lexical processing drives motor simulation. In proceedings of the Twenty-Seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
  122. Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830-846.
  123. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory, in E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381-403). New York: Academic Press.
  124. Tulving, E. (1995). Organization of memory: Quo vadis? In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences (pp. 839-847). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  125. Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352-373.
  126. Tyler, L. K., Stamatakis, E. A., Dick, E., Bright, P., Fletcher, P., & Moss, H. (2003). Objects and their actions: Evidence for a neurally distributed semantic system. NeuroImage, 18, 542-557.
  127. Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  128. Van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Perceptual processing affects conceptual processing. Cognitive Science, 32, 579-590.
  129. Van den Bergh, O., Vrana, S., & Eelen, P. (1990). Letters from the heart : affective categorization of letter combinations in typists and nontypists. Journal of Experimental Psychology :Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(6), 1153-1161.
  130. Vermeulen, N., Niedenthal, P.M., & Luminet, O. (2007). Switching between sensory and affective systems incurs processing costs, Cognitive Science, 31, 183-192.
  131. Versace, R., Labeye, E., Badard, G., & Rose, M. (2008). The contents of long-term memory and the emergence of knowledge. European journal of cognitive psychology, 1-39.
  132. Versace, R., Nevers, B., & Padovan, C. (2002). La mémoire dans tous ses états. Marseille : Solal.
  133. Versace, R., & Rose, M. (2007). The role of emotion in multimodal integration. Current Psychology Letters, Behavior, Brain, and Cognition, 21, 13-22.
  134. Wang, Y., & Morgan, W. P. (1992). The effect of imagery perspectives on the psychophysiological responses to imagined exercise. Behavioral Brain Research, 52, 167-174.
  135. Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2000).Memory’s echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(20), 11125-11129.
  136. Whittlesea, B. W. A. (1987). Preservation of specific experiences in the representation of general knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,13, 3-17.
  137. Whittlesea, B. W. A. (1989). Selective attention, variable processing, and distributed representation: Preserving particular experiences of general structures. In R. G. M. Morris (Ed.), Parallel distributed processing: Implications for psychology and neurobiology. Oxford, England: University Press.
  138. Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J-P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655-664.
  139. Zwaan, R. A., Madden, C. J., Yaxley, R. H., & Aveyard, M. E. (2004). Moving words: Dynamic representations in language comprehension. Cognitive Science, 28, 611-619.
  140. Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., & Yaxley, R. (2002). Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science, 13(2), 168-171.
  141. Zwaan, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2003). Spatial iconicity affects semantic relatedness judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 10(4), 954-958.
  142. Zwaan, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2004). Lateralization of object-shape information in semantic processing. Cognition 94, 35- 43.