Références bibliographiques

  1. Adlard, A. & Hazan,V. (1998). Speech perception abilities in children with developmental dyslexia.The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 153-177.
  2. Ahissar, M., Protopapas, A., Reid, M. & Merzenich, M. (2000). Auditory processing parallels reading abilities in adults. PNAS, 97, 6832-6837.
  3. Alario, F.-X., De Cara, B. & Ziegler, J. (2007). Automatic activation of phonology in silent reading is parallel: evidence from beginning and skilled readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 205-219.
  4. Altmann, G. (1997). The ascent of Babel: an exploration of language, mind, and understanding. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  5. Álvarez, C., Carreiras, M. & De Vega, M. (2000). Syllable-frequency effect in visual word recognition: evidence of sequential-type processing. Psícologica, 21, 341-374.
  6. Álvarez, C., Carreiras, M. & Perea, M. (2004). Are syllables phonological units in visual word recognition? Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 427-452.
  7. Álvarez, C., Carreiras, M. & Taft, M. (2001). Syllables and morphemes: contrasting frequency effects in Spanish. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 545-555.
  8. American Psychiatric Association (2004). Troubles des apprentissages. In DSM-IV-TR. Manuel Diagnostique et Statistique des Troubles Mentaux (pp. 56-65). Paris: Masson.
  9. Amitay, S., Ben Yehudah, G., Banai, K. & Ahissar, M. (2002). Disabled readers suffer from visual and auditory impairments but not from a specific magnocellular deficit. Brain, 125, 2272-2285.
  10. Anderson, J. (1983). The architecture of cognition. New York: Academic Press.
  11. Ans, B., Carbonnel, S. & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist multipletrace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review, 105, 678-723.
  12. Anthony, J. & Lonigan, C. (2004). The nature of phonological awareness: converging evidence from four studies of preschool and early grade school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 43-55.
  13. Anthony, J., Lonigan, C., Burgess, S., Driscoll, K., Philips, B. & Cantor, B. (2002). Structure of preschool phonological sensitivity: overlapping sensitivity to rhyme, words, syllables, and phonemes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 65-92.
  14. Anthony, J., Lonigan, C., Driscoll, K., Phillips, B. & Burgess, S. (2003). Preschool phonological sensitivity: a quasi-parallel progression of word structure units and cognitive operations. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 470-487.
  15. Arnbak, E. & Elbro, C. (2000). The effects of morphological awareness training on the reading and spelling skills of young dyslexics. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 44, 229-251.
  16. Bailey, C., Manis, F., Pedersen, W. & Seidenberg, M. (2004). Variation among developmental dyslexics: evidence from a printed-word-learning task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 125-154.
  17. Baldeweg, T., Richardson, A., Witkins, S., Foale, C. & Gruzelier, J. (1999). Impaired auditory frequency discrimination in dyslexia detected with mismatch evoked potentials. Annals of Neurology, 45, 495-503.
  18. Ballaz, C., Marendaz, C. & Valdois, S. (1999). Syllables as visual units: evidences from dyslexic and deaf children. Proceedings of the XI th Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology, Ghent, Belgium, 1st – 4th September.
  19. Balota, D., Cortese, M., Sergent-Marshall, S., Spieler, D. & Yap, M. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General., 133, 283-316.
  20. Banel, M.-H., Frauenfelder, U. & Perruchet, P. (1998). Contributions des indices métriques à l’apprentissage d’un langage artificial. XXII èmes Journées d’Études sur la Parole. Martigny.
  21. Bastien-Toniazzo, M., Magnan, A. & Bouchafa, H. (1996). Une étude longitudinale des stratégies d’apprentissage de la correspondance grapho-phonologie en français. Revue de Psychologie de l’Éducation, 2, 37-65.
  22. Bastien-Toniazzo, M., Magnan, A. & Bouchafa, H. (1999). Nature des représentations du langage écrit aux débuts de l’apprentissage de la lecture: un modèle interprétatif. Journal International de Psychologie, 34, 43-58.
  23. Bedoin N. (2003). Sensitivity to voicing similarity in printed stimuli: effect of a training program in dyslexic children. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 514-546.
  24. Bedoin, N. & Dissard, P. (2002). Sonority and syllabic structure in reading: differences between French and English readers. Current Psychology Letters: Brain, Behaviour and Cognition, 8, 67-83.
  25. Bell, A. & Hooper, J. (1978). Issues and evidence in syllabic phonology. In A. Bell & J. Hooper (Eds.), Syllables and segments. Amsterdam (pp. 3-24). North-Holland Publishing Co.
  26. Ben-Yehudah, G., Sackett, E., Malchi-Ginzberg, L. & Ahissar, M. (2001). Impaied temporal contrast sensitivity in dyslexics is specific to retain-and-compare paradigms. Brain, 124, 1381-1395.
  27. Berent, I. & Perfetti, C. (1995). A ROSE is a REEZ: the 2-cycles model of phonology assembly in reading English. Psychological Review, 102, 146-184.
  28. Berg, T. (1989). On the internal structure of polysyllabic monomorphemic words: the case for superrimes. Studia Linguistica, 43, 5-32.
  29. Bernhardt, B. & Stemberger, J. (1998). Handbook of phonological development from the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. San Diego: Academic Press.
  30. Bertoncini, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Jusczyk, P., Kennedy, L. & Mehler, J. (1988). An investigation of young infants’ perceptual representations of speech sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 21-33.
  31. Bijeljac-Babic, R., Bertoncini, J. & Mehler, J. (1993). How do four-day-old infants categorize multisyllabic utterances? Developmental Psychology, 29, 711-721
  32. Bijeljac-Babic, R., Millogo, V., Farioli, F. & Grainger, J. (2004). A developmental investigation of word length effects in reading using a new on-line word identification paradigm. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 411-431.
  33. Bishop, D., Carlyon, R., Deeks, J. & Bishop, S. (1999). Auditory temporal processing impairment. Neither necessary nor sufficient for causing language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1295-1310.
  34. Blevins, J. (1995). The syllable in phonological theory. In J. Goldsmith (Ed.), The handbook of phonological theory (pp. 206-244). Oxford: Blackwell.
  35. Blevins, J. (2004). Evolutionary phonology. The emergence of sound patterns. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  36. Boden, C. & Giaschi, D. (2007). M-stream deficits and reading-related visual processes in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 346-366.
  37. Boder, E. (1973). Developmental dyslexia: a diagnostic approach based on three atypical reading-spelling patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15, 663-687.
  38. Boets, B., Ghesquière, P., Wieringen, A. & Wouters, J. (2007). Speech perception in preschoolers at family risk for dyslexia: relations with low-level auditory processing and phonological ability. Brain and Language, 101, 19-30.
  39. Bogliotti, C., Messaoud-Galusi, S. & Serniclaes, W. (2002). Relations entre la perception catégorielle de la parole et l’apprentissage de la lecture. XXIV èmes Journées d’Études sur la Parole. Nancy.
  40. Bonin, P., Chalard, M., Méot, A. & Fayol, M. (2001). Age-of-acquisition and word frequency in the lexical decision task: further evidence from the French language. Current Psychology of Cognition, 20, 401-443.
  41. Bonin, P. & Fayol, M. (2000). Writing words from pictures: What representations are activated and when? Memory & Cognition, 28, 677-689.
  42. Bonin, P., Pacton, S. & Fayol, M. (2001). La production verbale écrite: évidence en faveur d’une (relative) autonomie de l’écrit. Psychologie Française, 46, 77-88.
  43. Booth, J., Perfetti, C. & MacWhinney, B. (1999). Quick, automatic and general activation of orthographic and phonological representations in young readers. Developmental Psychology, 35, 3-19.
  44. Bosman, A. & De Groot, A. (1995). Evidence for assembled phonology in beginning and fluent readers as assessed with the first-letter-naming task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 234-259.
  45. Bosman, A. & De Groot, A. (1996). Phonologic mediation is fundamental to reading: evidence from beginning readers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 49, 715-744.
  46. Bosse, M.-L., Tainturier, M. & Valdois, S. (2006). Developmental dyslexia: the visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198-230.
  47. Bowey, J. (2002). Reflections on onset-rime and phoneme sensitivity as predictors of beginning word reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 29-40.
  48. Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read. A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419-421.
  49. Bradley, D., Sánchez-Casas, R. & García-Albea, J. (1993). The status of the syllable in the perception of Spanish and English. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 197-233.
  50. Brand, M., Rey, A. & Peereman, R. (2003). Where is the syllable priming effect in visual word recognition? Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 435-443.
  51. Breier, J., Fletcher, J., Denton, C. & Gray, C. (2004). Categorical perception of speech stimuli in children at risk for reading difficulty. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 152-170.
  52. Breier, J., Gray, L., Fletcher, J., Diehl, R., Klaas, P., Foorman, B. & Molis, M. (2001). Perception of voice and tone onset time continua in children with dyslexia with and without attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 245-270.
  53. Breier, J., Gray, L., Fletcher, J., Foorman, B. & Klaas, P. (2002). Perception of speech and nonspeech stimuli by children with and without reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 226-250.
  54. Breitmeyer, B. (1980). Unmasking visual masking: a look at the “why” behind the veil of the “how”. Psychological Review, 87, 52-69.
  55. Brent, M. & Cartwright, T. (1996). Distributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation. Cognition, 61, 93-125.
  56. Brown, G. & Deavers, R. (1999). Units of analysis in nonword reading: evidence from children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 208-242.
  57. Bruck, M., Treiman R. & Caravolas, M. (1995). The syllable’s role in the processing of the spoken English: evidence from a nonword comparison task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 469-479.
  58. Bruck, M., Genesee, F. & Caravolas, M. (1997). A cross linguistic study of early literacy acquisition. In B. Blachmann (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: implications for early intervention (pp. 145-162). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  59. Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C., Frith, C. & Frith, U. (1999). Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: a search for Wernicke’s Wortschatz? Brain, 122, 1901-1917.
  60. Brysbaert, M. (2001). Prelexical phonological coding of visual words in Dutch: automatic after all. Memory & Cognition, 29, 765-773.
  61. Burr, D., Morrone, M. & Ross, J. (1994). Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements. Nature, 371, 511-513.
  62. Bus, A. & Van Ijzendoorn, M. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: a meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403-414.
  63. Calmus, C. (2007). Le role de la syllable chez l’apprenti lecteur de langue maternelle française. Thèse de l’Université Lumière Lyon 2 – France.
  64. Caravolas, M., Kessler, B., Hulme, C. & Snowling, M. (2005). Effects of orthographic consistency, frequency, and letter knowledge on children’s vowel spelling development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 307-321.
  65. Carreiras, M., Álvarez, C. & De Vega, M. (1993). Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 766-780.
  66. Carreiras, M., Ferrand, L., Grainger, J. & Perea, M. (2005a). Sequential effects of phonological priming in visual word recognition. Psychological Science, 16, 585–589.
  67. Carreiras, M., Mechelli, A. & Price, C. (2006). Effect of word and syllable frequency on activation during lexical decision and reading aloud. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 963-972.
  68. Carreiras, M. & Perea, M. (2002). Masked priming effects with syllabic neighbors in a lexical decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1228-1242.
  69. Carreiras, M. & Perea, M. (2004a). Naming pseudowords in Spanish: effects of syllable frequency. Brain and Language, 90, 393-400.
  70. Carreiras, M. & Perea, M. (2004b). Effets de la fréquence du voisinage syllabique dans la reconnaissance des mots écrits et la lecture: comparaisons inter-tâches. In L. Ferrand & J. Grainger (Eds.), Psycholinguistique Cognitive. Essais en l’honneur de Juan Seguí (pp. 233-251). Bruxelles: DeBoeck.
  71. Carreiras, M., Perea, M. & Grainger, J. (1997). Effects of orthographic neighbourhood in visual word recognition: cross-task comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 857-871.
  72. Carreiras, M., Vergara, M. & Barber, H. (2005b). Early event-related potential effects of syllabic processing during visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1803-1817.
  73. Casalis, S. (1995). Lecture et dyslexies de l’enfant. Paris : Septentrion.
  74. Casalis, S., Colé, P. & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 114-138.
  75. Castles, A. & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91, 77-111.
  76. Castles, A., Datta, H., Gayan, J. & Olson, R. (1999). Varieties of reading disorder: genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 73-94.
  77. Chait, M., Eden, G., Poeppel, D., Simon, J., Hill, D. & Flowers, L. (2007). Delayed detection of tonal targets in background noise in dyslexia. Brain and Language, 102, 80-90.
  78. Chaix, Y., Albaret, J.-M., Brassard, C., Cheuret, E., De Castelnau, P., Benesteau, J., Karsenty, C. & Démonet, J.-F. (2007). Motor impairment in dyslexia: the influence of attention disorders. European Journal of Pædiatric Neurology, 11, 368-374.
  79. Chen, J.-Y., Dell, G. & Chen, T.-M. (2002). Word form encoding in Mandarin Chinese as assessed by the implicit priming paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 751-781.
  80. Chéreau, C., Gaskell, M. & Dumay, N. (2007). Reading spoken words: orthographic effects in auditory priming. Cognition, 102, 341-360.
  81. Chiappe, P., Stringer, R., Siegel, L. & Stanovich, K. (2002). Why the timing deficit hypothesis does not explain reading disability in adults. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 73-107.
  82. Cholin, J., Levelt, W. & Schiller, N. (2006). Effects of syllable frequency in speech production. Cognition, 99, 205-235.
  83. Cholin, J., Schiller, N. & Levelt, W. (2004). The preparation of syllables in speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 47-61.
  84. Clements, G. (1990). The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. In M. Beckman & J. Kingston (Eds.), Papers in phonology I: between the grammar and the physics of speech (pp. 283-333). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  85. Clements, G. & Keyser, S. J. (1983). CV phonology: a generative theory of the syllable. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  86. Cohen, L. & Dehaene, S. (2004). Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area. NeuroImage, 22, 466-476.
  87. Cohen, J., McWhinney, B., Flatt, M. & Provost, J. (1993). PsyScope: an interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosch computers. Behavioural Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 25, 257-271.
  88. Colé, P., Magnan, A. & Grainger, J. (1999). Syllable-sized units in visual word recognition: evidence from skilled and beginning readers of French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 507-532.
  89. Colé, P., Royer, C., Leuwers, C. & Casalis, S. (2004). Les connaissances dérivationnelles et l’apprentissage de la lecture chez l’apprenti-lecteur français du CP au CE2. L’Année Psychologique, 104, 701-750.
  90. Colé, P. & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (1999). Traitement syllabique au cours de la reconnaissance de mots écrits chez des enfants dyslexiques, lecteurs en retard, et normo-lecteurs de 11 ans. Rééducation Orthophonique, 4, 323-360.
  91. Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151-216). London: Academic Press.
  92. Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P. & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589-608.
  93. Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 535–555). London: Academic Press.
  94. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R. & Ziegler, J. C. (2001). DRC: A computational model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204-256.
  95. Content, A., Dumay, N. & Frauenfelder, U. (2000). The role of syllable structure in lexical segmentation: helping listeners avoid mondegreens. In A. Cutler, J. McQueen & R. Zondervan (Eds.), Spoken word access processes (pp. 39-42). Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen: The Netherlands.
  96. Content, A. & Frauenfelder, U. (2002). La syllable comme unité de perception de la parole: un état de la question. XXIVèmes Journées d’Études sur la Parole. Nancy.
  97. Content, A., Kearns, R. & Frauenfelder, U. (2001b). Boundaries versus onsets in syllabic segmentation. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 177-199.
  98. Content, A., Meunier, C., Kearns, R. & Frauenfelder, U. (2001a). Sequence detection in pseudowords in French: where is the syllable effect? Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 609-636.
  99. Content, A., Mousty, P. & Radeau, M. (1990). Brulex: une base de données lexicales informatisées pour le français écrit et parlé. L’Année Psychologique, 90, 551-566.
  100. Content, A. & Zesiger, P. (1999). L’acquisition du langage écrit. In J.-A. Rondal & X. Séron (Eds.), Troubles du langage. Bases théoriques, diagnostic et rééducation (pp. 179-209). Liège: Mardaga.
  101. Conrad, M. & Jacobs, A. (2004). Replicating syllable frequency effects in Spanish in German: one more challenge to computational models of visual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 369-390.
  102. Conrad, M., Jacobs, A. & Grainger, J. (2007). Phonology as the source of the syllable frequency effects in visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 35, 974-983.
  103. Conrad, M., Stenneken, P. & Jacobs, A. (2006). Associated or dissociated effects of syllable frequency in lexical decision and naming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 339-345.
  104. Cornelissen, P., Hansen, P., Bradley, L. & Stein, J. (1996). Analysis of perceptual confusions between nine sets of consonant-vowel sounds in normal and dyslexic adults. Cognition, 59, 275-306.
  105. Cornelissen, P., Hansen, P., Gilchrist, Cormack, Essex & Frankish (1998). Coherent motion detection and letter position encoding. Vision Research, 38, 2181-2191.
  106. Cornelissen, P., Richardson, A., Mason, A., Fowler, S. & Stein, J. (1995). Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measured at photopic luminance levels in dyslexics and controls. Vision Research, 35, 1483-1494.
  107. Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. & Gugliotta, M. (1995). Visual and phonological determinants of misreadings in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 1-20.
  108. Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I., Katz, L. & Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Apllied Psycholinguistics, 9, 1-16.
  109. Costa, A. & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1998). Abstract syllabic structure in language production: evidence from Spanish. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 886-903.
  110. Cunningham, J., Nicol, T., Zecker, S., Bradlow, A. & Kraus, N. (2001). Neurobiologic responses to speech in noise in children with learning problems: deficits and strategies for improvement. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 758-767.
  111. Curtin, S., Mintz, T. & Christiansen, M. (2005). Stress changes the representational landscape: evidence from word segmentation. Cognition, 96, 233-262.
  112. Cutler, A. (1997). The syllable’s role in the segmentation of stress languages. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12(5/6), 839-845.
  113. Cutler, A., McQueen, J., Norris, D. & Somejuan, A. (2001). The roll of the silly ball. In E. Dupoux et al. (Eds.), Language, brain and cognitive development: essays in honor of Jacques Mehler (pp. 181-194). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  114. Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. & Seguí, J. (1983). A language specific comprehension strategy. Nature, 304, 141-159.
  115. Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. & Seguí, J. (1986). The syllable’s differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 385-400.
  116. Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. & Seguí, J. (1989). Limits on bilingualism. Nature, 340, 229-230.
  117. Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D. & Seguí, J. (1992). The monolingual nature of speech segmentation by bilinguals. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 381-410.
  118. Cutler, A. & Norris, D. (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 113-121.
  119. Danon-Boileau, L. & Barbier, D. (2001). Play-On: un logiciel d’entraînement à la lecture. Paris: Audivi-Média.
  120. De Cara, B. & Goswami, U. (2003). Phonological neighbourhood density effects in a rhyme awareness task in 5-year-old children. Journal of Child Language, 30, 695-710.
  121. DeFries, J., Fulker, D. & Labuda, M. (1987). Evidence for a genetic aetiology in reading disability of twins. Nature, 329, 537-539.
  122. Dehaene, S. (2007). Les neurones de la lecture. Paris: Odile Jacob.
  123. Delahaie, M., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Serniclaes, W., Billard, C., Tichet, J., Pointeau, S. & Vol, S. (2004a). Les habiletés de perception des sons de la parole d’une population d’enfants prélecteurs. ANAE , 77, 13-23.
  124. Delahaie, M., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Serniclaes, W., Billard, C., Tichet, J., Pointeau, S. & Vol, S. (2004 b). Perception catégorielle dans une tâche de discrimination de phonèmes et apprentissage de la lecture. Données longitudinales: 5 à 7 ans. Revue Française de Pédagogie , 147, 91-105
  125. Delattre, P. (1965). Comparing the phonetic featur es of English, German, Spanish and French. Heidelberg: Julius Groos Verlag.
  126. Dell, G. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentences production. Psychological Review, 93, 283-321.
  127. Dell, F. (1995). Consonant clusters and phonological syllables in French. Lingua, 95, 5-26.
  128. De Luca, M., Borrelli, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D. & Zoccolotti, P. (2002). Eye movement patterns in linguistic and non linguistic tasks in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 80, 617-626.
  129. Demb, J., Boynton, G. & Heeger, D. (1997). Brain activity in visual cortex predicts individual differences in reading performance. PNAS, 94, 13363-13366.
  130. Demb, J., Boynton, G. & Heeger, D. (1998a). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of early visual pathways in dyslexia. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 6939-6951.
  131. Demb, J., Boynton, G., Best, M. & Heeger, D. (1998b). Psychophysical evidence for a magnocellular pathway deficit in dyslexia. Vision Research, 38, 1555-1559.
  132. Démonet, J.-F. (2002). Apports de la neuro-imagerie fonctionnelle à la compréhension des dysfonctionnements cérébraux dans les dyslexies développementales. In R. Cheminal & V. Brun (Eds.), Les dyslexies (pp. 23-34). Masson : Paris.
  133. Démonet, J.-F., Fiez, J., Paulesu, E., Petersen, S. & Zatorre, R. (1996). PET studies of phonological processing: a critical reply to Poeppel. Brain and Language, 28, 352-385.
  134. Démonet, J.-F., Taylor, M. & Chaix, Y. (2004). Developmental dyslexia. The Lancet, 363, 1451-1460.
  135. Démonet, J.-F., Thierry, G. & Cardebat, D. (2005). Renewal of the neurophysiology of language: functional neuroimaging. Physiological Review, 85, 49-95.
  136. Derwing, B. (1992). A ‘pause-break’ task for eliciting syllable boundary
    judgments from literate and illiterate speakers: preliminary results for five
    diverse languages. Language and Speech, 35, 219–235.
  137. Deutsch, G., Dougherty, R., Bammer, R., Siok, W., Gabrieli, J. & Wandell, B. (2005). Children’s reading performance is correlated with white matter structure measured by diffusion tensor imaging. Cortex, 41, 354-363.
  138. De Weirdt, W. (1988). Speech perception and frequency discrimination in good and poor readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 163-183.
  139. Dijkstra, A., Roelofs, A. & Fieuws, S. (1995). Orthographic effects on phoneme monitoring. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 264-271.
  140. Doignon, N. & Zagar, D. (2005). Illusory conjunctions in French: the nature of sublexical units in visual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20, 443-464.
  141. Doignon, N. & Zagar, D. (2006). Les enfants en cours d’apprentissage de la lecture perçoivent-ils la syllable à l’écrit? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 258-274.
  142. Dominguez, A., De Vega, M. & Cuetos, F. (1997). Lexical inhibition from syllabic units in spanish visual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 401-422.
  143. Drieghe, D. & Brysbaert, M. (2002). Strategic effects in associative priming with words, homophones, and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 951-961.
  144. Dufor, O., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Démonet, J.-F. (2007). Top-down processes during auditory phoneme categorization in dyslexia: a PET study. NeuroImage, 1692-1707.
  145. Dufour, S., Peereman, R., Pallier, C. & Radeau, M. (2002). VoCoLex: une base de données lexicographiques sur les similarities phonologiques entre les mots français. L’Année Psychologique, 102, 725-746.
  146. Dumay, N., Content, A. & Frauenfelder, U. (2002a). Acoustic-phonetic cues to word boundary location: evidence from word spotting. XXIV èmes Journées d’Études sur la Parole. Nancy.
  147. Dumay, N., Frauenfelder, U. & Content, A. (2002b). The role of syllable in lexical segmentation in French: word-spotting data. Brain and Language, 81, 144-161.
  148. Duncan, L., Colé, P., Seymour, P. & Magnan, A. (2006). Differing sequences of metaphonological development in French and English. Journal of Child Language, 33, 369-399.
  149. Duncan, L. & Grieve, C. (2007). Line bissection in developmental dyslexia. Evidence of a left visual mini-neglect? Proceedings of the XV th Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology, Marseille, France, 29th August – 1st September.
  150. Duncan, L., Seymour, P. & Hill, S. (1997). How important are rhyme and analogy in beginning reading? Cognition , 63, 171-208.
  151. Duncan, L., Seymour, P. & Hill, S. (2000). A small to large unit progression in metaphonological awareness and reading? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A , 53, 1081-1104.
  152. Dupoux, E., Kakehi, K., Hirose, Y., Pallier, C., & Mehler, J. (1999). Epenthetic vowels in Japanese: A perceptual illusion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 25(6) , 1568-1578.
  153. Dupoux, E., Pallier, C., Sebastián, N. & Mehler, J. (1997). A destressing « deafness » in French? Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 406-421.
  154. Durand, J. & Laks, B. (2002). Phonetics, phonology and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.
  155. Écalle, J. (2003). Timé2: Test d’Identification de Mots Écrits pour enfants de 6 à 8 ans. Paris: ECPA.
  156. Écalle, J. (2006). Timé3: Test d’Identification de Mots Écrits pour enfants de 7 à 15 ans. Paris: Mot-à-Mot.
  157. Écalle, J. & Magnan, A. (2002a). L’apprentissage de la lecture. Fonctionnement et développement cognitifs. Paris: Armand Colin.
  158. Écalle, J. & Magnan, A. (2002b). The development of epiphonological and metaphonological processing at the start of learning to read: a longitudinal study. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 17, 47-62.
  159. Écalle, J. & Magnan, A. (2007). Development of phonological skills and learning to read in French. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 22, 153-167.
  160. Écalle, J., Magnan, A. & Bouchafa, H. (2002). Le développement des habiletés phonologiques avant et au cours de l’apprentissage de la lecture: de l’évaluation à la remédiation. Glossa, 82, 4-12.
  161. Eckert, M. (2004). Neuroanatomical markers for dyslexia: a review of dyslexia structural imaging studies. The Neuroscientist, 10, 362-371.
  162. Eckert, M., Leonard, C., Richards, T., Aylward, E., Thomson, J. & Berninger, V. (2003). Anatomical correlates of dyslexia: frontal and cerebellar findings. Brain, 126, 482-484.
  163. Eden, G., VanMeter, J., Rumsey, J., Maisog, J., Woods, R. & Zeffiro, T. (1996). Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature, 382, 66-69.
  164. Ehri, L. (1989). Apprendre à lire et à écrire des mots. In L. Rieben & C. Perfetti (Eds.), L’apprenti lecteur (pp. 103-128). Neuchâtel-Paris: Delachaux & Niestlé.
  165. Ehri, L. (1997). Apprendre à lire et apprendre à orthographier, c’est la même chose ou pratiquement la même chose. In L. Rieben, M. Fayol & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Des orthographes et leur acquisition (pp. 231-265). Paris: Delachaux & Niestlé.
  166. Ehri, L. (2005). learning to read words: theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading , 9, 167–188.
  167. Ehri, L., Nunes, S., Willows, D., Schuster, B., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z. & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: evidence from the National Reading Panel’s Meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287.
  168. Elbro, C. & Jensen, M. (2005). Quality of phonological representations, word learning, and phoneme awareness in dyslexic and normal readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 375-384.
  169. Encrevé, P. (1988). La liaison avec et sans enchaînement, phonologie tridimensionnelle et usages du français. Paris: Editions du Seuil.
  170. Fabbro, F., Pesenti, S., Facoetti, A., Bonanomi, M., Libera, L. & Lorusso, M. (2001). Callosal transfer in different subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 37, 65-73.
  171. Fabre, D. & Bedoin, N. (2003). Sensitivity to sonority for print processing in normal readers and dyslexic children. Current Psychology Letters: Brain, Behaviour and Cognition, Special Issue on Language Disorders and Reading Acquisition, 10, 1-8.
  172. Facoetti, A. Lorusso, M., Paganoni, P., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R. & Mascetti, G. (2003a). The time course of attentional focusing in dyslexic and normally reading children. Brain and Cognition, 53, 181-184.
  173. Facoetti, A., Lorusso, M., Paganoni, P., Umiltà, C. & Mascetti, G. (2003b). The role of visuo-spatial attention in developmental dyslexia: evidence from a rehabilitation study. Cognitive Brain Research, 15, 154-164.
  174. Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P. & Lorusso, M. (2000a). The spatial distribution of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 531-538.
  175. Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., Turatto, M., Marzola, V. & Mascetti, G. (2000b). Visual-spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 36, 109-123.
  176. Farmer, M. & Klein, R. (1995). The evidence for temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 460-493.
  177. Fawcett, A. & Nicolson, R. (2002). Children with dyslexia are slow to articulate a single speech gesture. Dyslexia, 8, 189-203.
  178. Fear, B., Cutler, A. & Butterfield, S. (1995). The strong/weak syllable distinction in English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97/3, 1893-1904.
  179. Ferrand, L. (2000). Reading aloud polysyllabic words and nonwords: the syllabic length effect re-examined. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(1), 142-148.
  180. Ferrand, L. (2001). Cognition et lecture. Bruxelles: DeBoeck.
  181. Ferrand, F. (2004). Nature des codes phonologiques actives au cours de la lecture silencieuse. In L. Ferrand & J. Grainger (Eds.), Psycholinguistique Cognitive. Essais en l’honneur de Juan Seguí (pp. 215-232). Bruxelles: DeBoeck.
  182. Ferrand, L. & Grainger, J. (2003). Homophonic interference effects in visual word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 403-419.
  183. Ferrand, L. & New, B. (2003). Syllabic length effects in visual word recognition and naming. Acta Psychologica, 113, 167-183.
  184. Ferrand, L. & Seguí, J. (1998). The syllable’s role in speech production: are syllables chunks, schemas, or both? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 523-528.
  185. Ferrand, L., Seguí, J. & Grainger, J. (1996). Masked priming of word and picture naming: the role of syllabic units. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 708-723.
  186. Ferrand, L., Seguí, J. & Humphreys, G. (1997). The syllable’s role in word naming. Memory & Cognition, 25(4), 458-470.
  187. Fiez, J. & Petersen, S. (1998). Neuroimaging studies of word reading. PNAS, 95, 14-21.
  188. Finney, S., Protopapas, A. & Eimas, P. (1996). Attentional allocation to syllables in American English. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 893-909.
  189. Fisher, S. & DeFries, J. (2002). Developmental dyslexia: genetic dissection of a complex trait. Nature Reviews, 3, 767-780.
  190. Floccia, J., Kolinsky, R., Dodane, C. & Morais, J. (2003). Discriminating spoken word in French: the role of the syllable and the CV phonological skeleton. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 241-267.
  191. France, S., Rosner, B., Hansen, P., Calvin, C., Talcott, J., Richardson, A. & Stein, J. (2000). Auditory frequency discrimination in adult developmental dyslexics. Perception ans Psychophysics, 64, 169-179.
  192. Frauenfelder, U., Segui, J. & Dijkstra, T. (1990). Lexical effects in phoneme processing: Facilitatory or inhibitory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 77-91.
  193. Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, J. Marshall & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301-330). London: Erlbaum.
  194. Frith, U. (1986). A developmental framework for developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 36, 69-81.
  195. Frith, U. (1999). Paradoxes in the definition of dyslexia. Dyslexia, 5, 192-214.
  196. Frith, U., Wimmer, H. & Landerl, K. (1998). Differences in phonological recoding in German- and English-speaking children. Scientific Studies in Reading, 2(1), 31-54.
  197. Frost, R. (1995). Phonological computation and missing vowels: mapping lexical involvement in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 398-408.
  198. Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: true issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71–99.
  199. Frost, R., Katz, L. & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: a multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 13(1), 104-115.
  200. Fudge, E. (1987). Branching structure within the syllable. Journal of Linguistics, 23, 359-377.
  201. Galaburda, A. (2002). Le cerveau et les sons : les leçons des rongeurs « dyslexiques ». In E. Dupoux (Ed.), Les langages du cerveau (pp. 443-457). Paris: Odile Jacob.
  202. Galaburda, A., LoTurco, J., Ramus, F., Fitch, R. & Rosen, G. (2006). From genes to behaviour in developmental dyslexia. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1213-1217.
  203. Galaburda, A., Menard, M. & Rosen, G. (1994). Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia. PNAS, 91, 8010-8013.
  204. Galaburda, A., Sherman, G., Rosen, G., Aboitiz, F. & Geschwind, N. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18, 222-233.
  205. Gaskell, M., Spinelli, E. & Meunier, F. (2002). Perception of resyllabification in French. Memory and Cognition, 30, 798-810.
  206. Génard, N., Mousty, P., Content, A., Alégria, J., Leybaert, J. & Morais, J. (1998). Methods to establish subtypes of developmental dyslexia. In P. Reitsma & L. Verhoeven (eds.), Problems and interventions in literacy development (pp. 163-176). Dordrecht : Kluwer.
  207. Geudens, A. & Sandra, D. (1999). Onsets and rimes in a phonologically transparent orthography: differences between good and poor beginning readers of Dutch. Brain and Language, 68, 284-290.
  208. Geudens, A. & Sandra, D. (2003). Beyond implicit phonological knowledge: no support for an onset-rime structure in children’s explicit phonological awareness. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 157-182.
  209. Geudens, A., Sandra, D. & Martensen, H. (2005). Rhyming words and onset-rime constituents: an inquiry into structural breaking points and emergent boundaries in the syllable. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 366-387.
  210. Giraud, K., Démonet, J.-F., Habib, M., Marquis, P., Chauvel, P. & Liégeois-Chauvel, C. (2005). Auditory evoked potential patterns to voiced and voiceless speech sounds in adult developmental dyslexics with persistent deficits. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1524-1534.
  211. Glushko, R. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674-691.
  212. Godfrey, J., Syrdal-Lasky, A., Millay, K. & Knox, C. (1981). Performance of dyslexic children on speech tests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 401-424.
  213. Goldman, J.-P., Content, A. & Frauenfelder, U. (1996). Comparaison des structures syllabiques en français et en anglais. XXI èmes Journées d’Études sur la Parole. Avignon (pp. 119-122).
  214. Goldrick, M. (2004). Phonological features and phonotactic constraints in speech production. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 586-603.
  215. Goldsmith, J. (1990). Autosegmental and metrical phonology. Oxford: Blackwell.
  216. Gombert, J.-É. (1990). Le développement métalinguistique. Paris: PUF.
  217. Gombert, J.-É. (1992). Activité de lecture et activités associées. In M. Fayol, J.-É. Gombert, P. Lecocq, L., Sprenger-Charolles & D. Zagar (Eds.), Psychologie cognitive de la lecture (pp.107-140). Paris: PUF.
  218. Gombert, J.-É. (1995). Recherches sur l’utilisation des analogies orthographiques par l’apprenti lecteur. Glossa, 46/47, 40-50.
  219. Gombert, J.-É. (2003). Learning to read: which codes are concerned? At which level of cognitive control? In R. Joshi, C. Leong & B. Kaczmarek (Eds.), Literacy acquisition. The role of phonology, morphology and orthography (pp. 3-13). Amsterdam: IOS Press.
  220. Gombert, J.-É., Bryant, P. & Warrick, N. (1997). Les analogies dans l’apprentissage de la lecture et de l’orthographe. In L. Rieben, M. Fayol & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Des orthographes et leur acquisition (pp. 319-334). Paris: Delachaux & Niestlé.
  221. Gombert, J.-É. & Colé, P. (2000). Activités métalinguistiques, lecture et illettrisme. In M. Kail & M. Fayol (Eds.), L’acquisition du langage: le langage en développement au-delà de 3 ans (pp. 117-150). Paris: PUF.
  222. Goslin, J. & Floccia, C. (2007). Comparing French syllabification in preliterate children and adults. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 341-367.
  223. Goslin, J., Grainger, J. & Holcomb, P. (2006). Syllable frequency effects in French visual word recognition: an ERP study. Brain Research, 1115, 121-134.
  224. Goswami, U. (1986). Children’s use of analogy in learning to read: a developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 72-83.
  225. Goswami, U. (1990). Phonological priming and orthographic analogies in reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 323-340.
  226. Goswami, U. (1993). Toward an interactive analogy model of reading development: decoding vowel graphemes in beginning reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 443-475.
  227. Goswami, U. (1995). Phonological development and reading by analogy: what is analogy, and what is it not? Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 139-145.
  228. Goswami, U. (1999). Causal connections in beginning reading: the importance of rhyme. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 217-240.
  229. Goswami, U. (2002). Phonology, reading development and dyslexia: a cross-linguistic perspective. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 1-23.
  230. Goswami, U. & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  231. Goswami, U. & East, M. (2000). Rhyme and analogy in beginning reading: conceptual and methodological issues. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 63-93.
  232. Goswami, U., Gombert, J.-É., Fraca De Barrera, L. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: nonsense word reading in English, French and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 19-52.
  233. Goswami, U., Porpodas, C. & Wheelwright, S. (1997). Children’s orthographic representations in English and Greek. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 3, 273-292.
  234. Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S. & Scott, S. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: a new hypothesis. PNAS, 99, 10911-10916.
  235. Goswami, U., Ziegler, J., Dalton, L. & Schneider, W. (2001). Pseudohomophone effects and phonological recoding procedures in reading development in English and German. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 648-664.
  236. Goswami, U., Ziegler, J., Dalton, L. & Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: how flexible is the choice of reading units. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235-247.
  237. Gotor, A., Perea, M. & Algarabel, S. (1995). Effects of syllable structure in visual word recognition: evidence from a masked priming technique. Proceedings of the VIII th European Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology,Roma, Italia.
  238. Goulandris, N. & Snowling, M. (1991). Visual memory deficits: a plausible cause of developmental dyslexia? Evidence from a single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 127-154.
  239. Grainger, J. (2008). Cracking the orthographic code: an introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(1), 1-35.
  240. Grainger, J., Bouttevin, S., Truc, C., Bastien, M. & Ziegler, J. (2003). Word superiority, pseudoword superiority, and learning to read: a comparison of dyslexic and normal readers. Brain and Language, 87, 432-440.
  241. Grainger, J. & Jacobs, A. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103, 518–565.
  242. Grainger, J., Muneaux, M., Farioli, F. & Ziegler, J. (2005). Effects of phonological and orthographic neighbourhood density interact in visual word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 58A (6), 981-998.
  243. Grainger, J., O’Regan, J., Jacobs, A. & Seguí, J. (1989). On the role of competing word units in visual word recognition: the neighborhood frequency effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 189-195.
  244. Grainger, J., SPinelli, E. & Ferrand, L. (2000). Effects of baseword frequency and orthographic neighborhood size in pseudohomophone naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 88-102.
  245. Griffiths, S. & Frith, U. (2002). Evidence for an articulatory deficit in adult dyslexics. Dyslexia, 8, 14-21.
  246. Grigorenko, E. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: an update on genes, brains and environments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 91-125.
  247. Grigorenko, E., Wood, F., Golovyan, L., Meyer, M., Romano, C. & Pauls, D. (2003). Continuing the search for dyslexia genes on 6p. American Journal of Human Genetics, 118B, 89-98.
  248. Gross, J., Treiman, R. & Inman, J. (2000). The role of phonology in a letter detection task. Memory & Cognition, 28, 349-357.
  249. Habib, M. (1997). Dyslexie : le cerveau singulier. Marseille: Solal.
  250. Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: an overview and working hypothesis. Brain, 123, 2373-2399.
  251. Habib, M. (2002). Effet d’un entraînement phonologique utilisant de la parole temporellement modifiée chez des enfants souffrant de dyslexie développementale. XXIV èmes Journées d’Etudes sur la Parole. Nancy.
  252. Habib, M. (2003). Rewiring the dyslexic brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 330-333.
  253. Habib, M., Espesser, R., Rey, V., Giraud, K., Bruas, P. & Gres, C. (1999). Training dyslexics with acoustically modified speech: evidence of improved phonological performance. Brain and Cognition, 40, 143-146.
  254. Habib, M., Giraud, K., Rey, V. & Robichon, F. (1999). Neurobiologie du Langage. In J.-A. Rondal & X. Séron (Eds.), Troubles du langage. Bases théoriques, diagnostic et rééducation (pp. 11-55). Liège: Mardaga.
  255. Habib, M. & Rey, V. (2000). Dyslexie, dyslexies. Aix-en-Provence: PUP.
  256. Habib, M., Rey, V., Daffaure, V., Camps, R., Espesser, R., Joly-Pottuz, B. & Démonet, J.-F. (2002). Phonological training in children with dyslexia using temporally modified speech: a three-step pilot investigation. Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 289-308.
  257. Hallé, P., Chéreau, C. & Seguí, J. (2000). Where is the /b/ in « absurde » [apsyrd]? It is in French listener’s minds. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 618-639.
  258. Hallé, P., Seguí, J., Frauenfelder, U. & Meunier, C. (1998). Processing of illegal consonant clusters: a case of perceptual assimilation? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 592-608.
  259. Hämäläinen, J., Leppänen, P., Torppa, M., Müller, K. & Lyytinen, H. (2005). Detection of sound rise time by adults with dyslexia. Brain and Language, 94, 32-42.
  260. Hannula-Jouppi, K., Kaminen-Ahola, N., Taipale, M., Eklund, R., Nopola-Hemmi, J., Kääriäinen, H. & Kere, J. (2005). The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate for developmental dyslexia. Plos Genetics, 1, e50.
  261. Hansen, P., Stein, J., Orde, S., Winter, J. & Talcott, J. (2001). Are dyslexics’ visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function? NeuroReport, 12, 1527-1530.
  262. Hari, R. & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 525-532.
  263. Hari, R., Renvall, H. & Tanskanen, T. (2001). Left minineglect in dyslexic adults. Brain, 124, 1373-1380.
  264. Hari, R., Sääskilahti, A., Helenius, P. & Uutela, K. (1999). Non-impaired auditory phase-locking in dyslexic adults. NeuroReport, 10, 2347-2348.
  265. Hari, R., Valta, M. & Uutela, K. (1999). Prolonged attentional dwell time in dyslexic adults. Neuroscience Letters, 271, 202-204.
  266. Harm, M. & Seidenberg, M. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491-528.
  267. Harm, M.W. & Seidenberg, M.S. (2001) Are there Orthographic Impairments in Phonological Dyslexia?Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 71-92.
  268. Harm, W. & Seidenberg, M. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111, 662-720.
  269. Hatcher, P. & Hulme, C. (1999). Phonemes, rhymes and intelligence as predictors of children’s responsiveness to remedial reading instruction: evidence from a longitudinal intervention study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 130-153.
  270. Hazan, V. & Barrett, S. (2000) The development of phonemic categorisation in children aged 6 to 12. Journal of Phonetics, 28, 377-396.
  271. Heiervang, E., Hugdahl, K., Steinmetz, H., Smievoll, A., Stevenson, J., Lund, A., Ersland, L. & Lundervold, A. (2000). Planum temporale, planum parietale and dichotic listening in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1704-1713.
  272. Heiervang, E., Stevenson, J. & Hugdahl, K. (2002). Auditory processing in children with dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 931-938.
  273. Heilman, K., Voeller, K. & Alexander, A. (1996). Developmental dyslexia: a motor-articulatory feedback hypothesis. Annals of Neurology, 39, 407-412.
  274. Heim, S. & Keil, A. (2004). Large-scale neural correlates of developmental dyslexia. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 125-140.
  275. Helenius, P., Tarkiainen, A., Cornelissen, P., Hansen, P. & Salmelin, R. (1999). Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 476-483.
  276. Helenius, P., Uutela, K. & Hari, R. (1999). Auditory stream segregation in dyslexic adults. Brain, 122, 907-913.
  277. Hilaire-Debove, G. & Kehoe, M. (2004). Acquisition des consonnes finales (codas) chez les enfants francophones: des universaux aux spécificités de la langue maternelle. XXV èmes Journées d’Études sur la Parole, Fez.
  278. Hoien, T., Lundberg, I., Stanovich, K. & Bjaalid, I. (1995). Components of phonological awareness. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 171-188.
  279. Hooper, J. B. (1972). The syllable in phonological theory. Language, 48(3), 525-540.
  280. Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J. & Donohue, B. (1998). Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. PNAS, 95, 8939-8944.
  281. Houghton, G. & Zorzi, M. (2003). Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual-route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 115-162.
  282. Hulme, C. (2002). Phonemes, rimes and the mechanisms of early reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 58-64.
  283. Hulme, C., Hatcher, P., Nation, K., Brown, A., Adams, J. & Stuart, G. (2002). Phoneme awareness is a better predictor of early reading skill than onset-rime awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 2-28.
  284. Hulme, C., Muter, V. & Snowling, M. (1998). Segmentation does predict early progress in learning to read better than rhyme: a reply to Bryant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 39-44
  285. Hutzler, F., Bergmann, J., Conrad, M., Kronbichler, M., Stenneken, P. & Jacobs, A. (2004). Inhibitory effects of first syllable-frequency in lexical decision: an event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 372, 179-184.
  286. Hutzler, F., Conrad, M. & Jacobs, A. (2005). Effects of syllable frequency in lexical decision and naming: an eye movement study. Brain and Language, 92, 138-152.
  287. Hutzler, F., Kronbichler, M., Jacobs, A. & Wimmer, H. (2006). Perhaps correlational but not causal: no effect of dyslexic readers’ magnocellular system of their eye movements during reading. Neuropsychologia, 44, 637-648.
  288. Hutzler, F. & Wimmer, H. (2004). Eye movements of dyslexic children when reading in a regular orthography. Brain and Language, 89, 235-242.
  289. Hutzler, F., Ziegler, J., Perry, C., Wimmer, H. & Zorzi, M. (2004). Do current connectionist learning models account for reading development in different languages? Cognition, 91, 273-296.
  290. Iles, J., Walsh, V. & Richardson, A. (2000). Visual search in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 6, 163-177.
  291. INSERM (2007). Expertice Collective. Dyslexie, Dysorthographie, Dyscalculie. Bilan des données scientifiques. Synthèse et recommendations. Paris: Les Éditions Inserm.
  292. Jacobs, A., Rey, A., Ziegler, J. & Grainger, J. (1998). MROMp: an interactive activation multiple readout model of orthographic and phonological processes in visual word recognition. In J. Grainger & A. Jacobs (Eds.), Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition (pp. 147-188). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  293. Jansma, B. & Schiller, N. (2004). Monitoring syllable boundaries during speech production. Brain and Language, 90, 311-317.
  294. Jared, D. (2002). Spelling-sound consistency and regularity effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 723-750.
  295. Jenner, A., Rosen, G. & Galaburda, A. (1999). Neural asymmetries in primary visual cortex of dyslexic and nondyslexic brains. Annals of Neurology, 46, 189-196.
  296. Joanisse, M., Manis, F., Keating, P. & Seidenberg, M. (2000). Language deficits in dyslexic children: speech perception, phonology and morphology. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 30-60.
  297. Juphard, A., Carbonnel, S., Ans, B. & Valdois, S. (2006). Length effect in reading and lexical decision: the multitrace memory model’s account. Current Psychology Letters: Behavior Brain & Cognition , 19(2), http://cpl.revues.org/document1005.html
  298. Jusczyk, P., Jusczyk, A., Kennedy, L., Schomberg, T. & Koenig, N. (1995). Young infants’ retention of information about bisyllabic utterances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 822-836
  299. Kahn, D. (1980). Syllable-based generalizations in English phonology. New York: Garland.
  300. Kandel, S., Álvarez, C. & Vallée, N. (2006). Syllables as processing units in handwriting production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 18-31.
  301. Kandel, S., Soler, O., Valdois, S. & Gros, C. (2006). Graphemes as motor units in the acquisition of writing skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 313-337.
  302. Kandel, S. & Valdois, S. (2005). The effect of orthographic regularity on children’s handwriting production. Current Psychology Letters: Brain, Behaviour and Cognition, 17. http://cpl.revues.org/document463.html
  303. Kandel, S. & Valdois, S. (2006a). Syllables as functional units in a copying task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 432-452.
  304. Kandel, S. & Valdois, S. (2006b). French and Spanish-speaking children use different visual and motor units during spelling acquisition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 531-561.
  305. Karmiloff, K. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Comment les enfants entrent dans le langage. Paris: Retz.
  306. Katz, L. & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: the orthographic depth hypothesis. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning. Advances in psychology (pp. 67-84). Oxford: North-Holland.
  307. Kaye, J. & Lowenstamm, J. (1984). De la syllabicité. In F. Dell, D. Hirst & J. Vergnaud (Eds.), Forme sonore du langage (pp. 123-159). Paris: Hermann.
  308. Kessler, B. & Treiman, R. (1997). Syllable structure and the distribution of phonemes in English syllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 295-311.
  309. King, C., Warrier, C., Hayes, E. & Kraus, N. (2002). Deficits in auditory brainstem pathway encoding of speech sounds in children with learning problems. Neuroscience Letters, 357, 207-210.
  310. Klein, M. (1993). La syllabe comme interface de la production et de la réception phonique. In B. Laks & M. Plénat (Eds.), De natura sonorum (pp. 101-143). Paris: PUV.
  311. Klingberg, T., Hedehus, M., Temple, E., Salz, T., Gabrieli, J., Moseley, M. & Poldrack, R. (2000). Microstructure of temporo-parietal white matter as a basis for reading ability: evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuron, 25, 493-500.
  312. Kolinsky, R., Morais, J. & Cluytens, M. (1995). Intermediate representations in spoken word recognition: evidence from word illusions. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 19-40.
  313. Kraus, N., McGee, T., Carrell, T., Zecker, S., Nicol, T. & Koch, D. (1996). Auditory neurophysiologic responses and discrimination deficits in children with learning problems. Science, 273, 971-973.
  314. Kronbichler, M., Hutzler, F. & Wimmer, H. (2002). Dyslexia: verbal impairments in the absence of magnocellular impairments. NeuroReport, 13, 617-620.
  315. Kronbichler, M., Wimmer, H., Staffen, W., Hutzler, F., Mair, A. & Ladurner, G. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: Gray matter abnormalities in the occipitotemporal cortex. Human Brain Mapping, doi:10.1002/hbm.20425.
  316. Kujala, T., Belitz, S., Tervaniemi, M. & Näätänen, R. (2003). Auditory sensory memory disorder in dyslexic adults as indexed by the mismatch negativity. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1323-1327.
  317. Kujala, T., Karma, K., Ceponiene, R., Belitz, S., Turkkila, P., Tervaniemi, M. & Näätänen, R. (2001). Plastic neural changes and reading improvement caused by audiovisual training in reading-impaired children. PNAS, 98, 10509-10514.
  318. Kujala, Myllyviita, Tervaniemi, Alho, Kallio & Näätanen, R. (2000). Basic auditory dysfunction in dyslexia as demonstrated by brain activity measurements. Psychophysiology, 37, 262-266.
  319. Kujala, T. & Näätänen, R. (2001). The mismatch negativity in evaluating central auditory dysfunction in dyslexia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 535-543.
  320. Kwantes, P. J., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (1999). Evidence for sequential processing in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 376-381.
  321. Lachmann, T., Berti, S., Kujala, T. & Schröger, E. (2005). Diagnostic subgroups of developmental dyslexia have different deficits in neural processing of tones and phonemes. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 56, 105-120.
  322. Laing, E. & Hulme, C. (1999). Phonological and semantic processes influence beginning readers’ ability to learn to read words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 183-207.
  323. Lamberts, K. (2005). Interpretation of orthographic uniqueness point effects in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 14-19.
  324. Landerl, K., Wimmer, H. & Frith, U. (1997). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia: evidence from german and English children. Cognition, 63, 315-334.
  325. Larsen, J., Høien, T., Lundberg, I. & Ødegaard, H. (1990). MRI evaluation of the planum temporale in adolescents with developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 39, 289-301.
  326. Leclercq, G., Content, A. & Frauenfelder, U. (2002). L’effet syllabique dans les mots et les pseudomots en français. XXIV èmes Journées d’Études sur la Parole. Nancy.
  327. Lecocq, P. (1991). Apprentissage de la lecture et dyslexie. Bruxelles: Mardaga.
  328. Lefavrais, P. (1965). Test de l’Alouette. Paris: ECPA.
  329. Lété, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Colé, P. (2004). Manulex: a grade-level lexical database from French elementary-school readers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 156-166.
  330. Levelt, W. (2001). Spoken word production: a theory of lexical access. PNAS, 98, 13464-13471.
  331. Levelt, W., Roelofs, A. & Meyer, A. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1-75.
  332. Levelt, C., Schiller, N. & Levelt, W. (2000). The acquisition of syllable types. Language Acquisition, 8, 237-264.
  333. Levelt, W. & Wheeldon, L. (1994). Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary? Cognition, 50, 239-269.
  334. Leybaert, J. & Content, A. (1995). Reading and spelling acquisition in two different teaching methods: A test of the independence hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 65-88.
  335. Liberman, A., Cooper, F., Shankweiler, D. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431-461.
  336. Liberman, I., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, W. & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201-212.
  337. Lindell, A., Nicholls, M. & Castles, A. (2003). The effect of orthographic uniqueness and deviation points on lexical decisions: evidence from unilateral and bilateral-redundant presentations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 287-307.
  338. Lindell, A., Nicholls, M., Kwantes, P. & Castles, A. (2005). Sequential processing in hemispheric word recognition: the impact of initial letter discriminability on the OUP naming effect. Brain and Language, 93, 160-172.
  339. Lorenzi, C., Dumont, A. & Füllgrabe, C. (2000). Use of temporal enveloppe cues by children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 1367-1379.
  340. Lorusso, M., Facoetti, A., Toraldo, A. & Molteni, M. (2005). Tachistoscopic treatment of dyslexia changes the distribution of visual-spatial attention. Brain and Cognition, 57, 135-142.
  341. Lukatela, G., Frost, S. & Turvey, M. (1998). Phonological priming by masked nonword primes in the lexical decision task. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 666-683.
  342. Lukatela, G. & Turvey, M. (1994a). Visual access is initially phonological: 1. Evidence from associative priming by words, homophones, and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 107–128.
  343. Lundberg, I., Frost, J. & Petersen, O. (1988). Effect of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263-284.
  344. Lupker, S., Perea, M. & Carreiras, M. (2008). Transposed-letter effects: consonants, vowels and letter frequency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(1), 93-116.
  345. Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Erskine, J., Guttorm, T., Laakso, M., Leppänen, P., Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A., Richardson, U. & Torppa, M. (2004). The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: birth to school age. Annals of Dyslexia , 54, 184-220.
  346. Macizo, P. & Van Petten, C. (2007). Syllable frequency in lexical decision and naming of English words. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 295-331.
  347. Magnan, A. & Écalle, J. (2001). Effets de congruence syllabique dans une tâche d’amorçage. In H. Paugam-Moisy, V. Nyckees & Caron-Pargue (Eds.), La cognition entre individu et société. Paris: Hermès Science Publication.
  348. Magnan, A. & Écalle, J. (2006). Audio-visual training in children with reading disabilities. Computers & Education, 46, 407-425.
  349. Magnan, A., Écalle, J. & Calmus, C. (in press). The effect of computer-aided learning in improving literacy skills in low-progress readers. In T. Scott & J. Livingstone (Eds.), Leading edge educational technology. Hauppauge: Nova Publishers.
  350. Magnan, A., Écalle, J. & Veuillet, E. (2005). Habiletés phonologiques, identification de mots écrits et déficits auditifs perceptifs chez les enfants dyslexiques : effet d'u entraînement audio-visuel. Revue Française de Pédagogie, 152, 29-40.
  351. Magnan, A., Écalle, J., Veuillet, E. & Collet, L. (2004). The effects of an audio-visual training program in dyslexic children. Dyslexia, 10, 131-140.
  352. Manis, F., McBride-Chang, C., Seidenberg, M., Keating, P., Doi, M., Munson, B. & Petersen, A. (1997). Are speech perception deficits associated with developmental dyslexia? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 211-235.
  353. Manis, F., Seidenberg, M., Doi, M., McBride-Chang, C. & Petersen, A. (1996). On the basis of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157-195.
  354. Marec-Breton, N., Gombert, J.-É. & Colé, P. (2005).Traitements morphologiques lors de la reconnaissance des mots écrits chez des apprenti-lecteurs. L’Année Psychologique, 105, 9-45.
  355. Marouby-Terriou, G. & Denhière, G. (2002). Identifier l’écrit: influence des connaissances infra-lexicales. Enfance, 54, 381-407.
  356. Marsh, G., Friedman, M., Welch, V. & Desberg, P. (1981). A cognitive developmental theory of reading acquisition. In G. MacKinnon & T. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: advances in theory and practice. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  357. Marshall, C., Snowling, M. & Bailey, P. (2001). Rapid auditory processing and phonological ability in normal readers and readers with dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 925-940.
  358. Marslen-Wilson, W. & Welsh, A. (1978). Processing interactions and lexical access during word-recognition in continuous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 29-63.
  359. Masterson, J., Hazan, V. & Wijayatilake, L. (1995). Phonemic processing problems in developmental phonological dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 233-259.
  360. Mathey, S., & Zagar, D. (2000). The neighborhood distribution effect in visual word recognition: words with single and twin neighbors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 184-205.
  361. Mathey, S. & Zagar, D. (2002). Lexical similarity in visual word recognition: the effect of syllabic neighborhood in French. Current Psychology Letters: Behavior Brain & Cognition, 8, 107-121.
  362. Mathey, S. & Zagar, D. (2006). The orthographic neighbourhood frequency effect in French: a letter-case manipulation study. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(2), 159-165.
  363. Mathey, S., Zagar, D., Doignon, N. & Seigneuric, A. (2006). The nature of the syllabic neighbourhood effects in French. Acta Psychologica, 123, 372-393.
  364. McAnally, K. & Stein, J. (1996). Auditory temporal coding in dyslexia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Biological Sciences, 263, 961-965.
  365. McAnally, K. & Stein, J. (1997). Scalp potentials evoked by amplitude-modulated tones in dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 939-945.
  366. McCandliss, B., Cohen, L. & Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 293-299.
  367. McClelland, J. & Rumelhart, D. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: 1. An account of the basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 357-407.
  368. McCrory, E., Frith, U., Brunswick, N. & Price, C. (2000). Abnormal functional activation during a simple word repetition task: a PET study of adult dyslexics. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 753-762.
  369. McCrory, E., Mechelli, A., Frith, U. & Price, C. (2005). More than words: a common neural basis for reading and naming deficits in developmental dyslexia? Brain, 128, 261-267.
  370. McQueen, J. (1998). Segmentation of continuous speech using phonotactics. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 21-46.
  371. Mechelli, A., Crinion, J., Long, S., Friston, K., Lambon Ralph, M., Patterson, K., McClelland, J. & Price, C. (2005). Dissociating reading processes on the basis of neural interactions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1753-1765.
  372. Mehler, J., Dommergues, J., Frauenfelder, U. & Seguí, J. (1981). The syllable’s role in speech segmentation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 298-305.
  373. Mehler, J. & Dupoux, E. (1995). Naître humain. Paris: Odile Jacob.
  374. Mehler, J. & Dupoux, E. (2004). Combien de syllables dans “psycholinguistique”? Discussions autour de Juan Seguí. In L. Ferrand & J. Grainger (Eds.), Psycholinguistique Cognitive. Essais en l’honneur de Juan Seguí (pp. 38-56). Bruxelles: DeBoeck.
  375. Mehler, J., Dupoux, E. & Seguí, J. (1990). Constraining models of lexical access: the onset of word recognition. In G. Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing (pp. 236-262). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  376. Menell, P., McAnally, K. & Stein, J. (1999). Psychophysical sensitivity and physiological response to amplitude modulation in adult dyslexic listeners. Journal of Specch, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 797-803.
  377. Merzenich, M., Jenkins, W., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S. & Tallal, P. (1996). Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science, 271, 77-81.
  378. Metsala, J., Stanovich, K. & Brown, G. (1998). Regularity effects and the phonological deficit model of reading disabilities: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 279-293.
  379. Meynadier, Y. (2001). La syllabe phonétique et phonologique: une introduction. Travaux Interdisciplinaires du Laboratoire Parole et Langage, 20, 91-148.
  380. Miller, S., Linn, L., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. & Jenkins, W. (1999). Entraînement à la parole et au langage acoustiquement modifiés: une relation entre l’entraînement à la discrimination auditive du mot et les mesures d’évolution du langage. Rééducation Orthophonique, 36, 159-182.
  381. Miles, E. (2000). Dyslexia may show a different face in different languages. Dyslexia, 6, 193-201.
  382. Mody, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Brady, S. (1997). Speech perception deficits in poor readers: auditory processing or phonological coding? Journal of Experimental Child PSychology, 64, 199-231.
  383. Morais, J. (1994). L’art de lire. Paris: Odile Jacob.
  384. Morais, J. (1995). Do orthographic and phonological peculiarities of alphabetically written languages influence the course of literacy acquisition? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 1-7.
  385. Morais, J. (2003). Levels of phonological representation in learning to read and in skilled reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 123-151.
  386. Morais, J., Content, A., Cary, L., Mehler, J. & Seguí, J. (1989). Syllabic segmentation and literacy. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 57-67.
  387. Muneaux, M., Ziegler, J., Truc, C., Thomson, J. & Goswami, U. (2004). Deficits in beat perception and dyslexia: evidence from French. NeuroReport, 15, 1-5.
  388. Murray, R. & Vennemann, T. (1983). Sound change and syllable structure in Germanic phonology. Language, 59, 514-528.
  389. Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. & Taylor, S. (1998). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 3-27.
  390. Nagarajan, S. (2002). Successive signal representation in noise in dyslexics. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, 459-461.
  391. Nagarajan, S., Mahncke, H., Salz, T., Tallal, P., Roberts, T. & Merzenich, M. (1999). Cortical auditory signal processing in poor-readers, PNAS, 96, 6483-6488.
  392. Nation, K. & Hulme, C. (1997). Phonemic segmentation, not onset-rime segmentation, predicts early reading and spelling skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 156-167.
  393. Nazzi, T., Dilley, L., Jusczyk, A., Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. & Jusczyk, P. (2005). English-learning infants’ segmentation of verbs from fluent speech. Language and Speech, 48, 279-298.
  394. Nazzi, T., Iakimova, G., Bertoncini, J., Frédonie, S. & Alcantara, C. (2006). Early segmentation of fluent speech by infants acquiring French: emerging evidence for cross linguistic differences. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 283-299.
  395. New, B. , Ferrand, L., Pallier, C. & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Re-examining word length effects in visual word recognition: new evidence from the English Lexicon Project. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 45-52.
  396. New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L. & Matos, R. (2001). Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE. L’Année Psychologique, 101, 447-462.
  397. Nicolson, R. & Fawcett, A. (2005). Developmental dyslexia, learning and the cerebellum. Journal of Neural Transmission Supplement, 69, 19-36.
  398. Nicolson, R., Fawcett, A. & Dean, P. (2001a). Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 508-511.
  399. Nicolson, R., Fawcett, A. & Dean, P. (2001b). Dyslexia, development and the cerebellum. Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 515-516.
  400. Nicolson, R., Fawcett, A., Berry, E., Jenkins, I., Dean, P. & Brooks, D. (1999). Association of abnormal cerebellar activation with motor learning difficulties in dyslexic adults. Lancet, 353, 1662-1667.
  401. Nittrouer, S. (1999). Do temporal processing deficits cause phonological processing problems? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 925-942.
  402. Nopola-Hemmi, J., Myllyluoma, B., Haltia, T., Taipale, M., Ollikainen, V., Ahonen, T., Voutilainen, A., Kere, J. & Widén, E. (2001). A dominant gene for developmental dyslexia on chromosome 3. Journal of Medical Genetics, 38, 658-664.
  403. Norris, D., McQueen, J. & Cutler, A. (1995). Competition and segmentation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 1209-1228.
  404. Norris, D., McQueen, J., Cutler, A. & Butterfield, S. (1997). The possible-word constraint in the segmentation of continuous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 34, 191-243.
  405. Ognjenovic, V., Lukatela, G., Feldman, L. & Turvey, M. (1983). Misreading by beginning readers of Serbo-Croatian, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 97-109.
  406. Oldfield, R. (1971). The assessment and the analysis of handedness: the Edinburg inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113.
  407. Organisation Mondiale de la Santé (1994). Troubles spécifiques du développement des acquisitions scolaires. In Classification Internationale des Maladies (pp. 132-135). Paris: Masson.
  408. Otake, T., Hatano, G., Cutler, A. & Mehler, J. (1993). Mora or syllable? Speech segmentation in Japanese. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 258-278.
  409. Pacton, S. , Perruchet, P., Fayol, M. & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning out of the lab: the case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401-426.
  410. Pallier, C., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Felguera, T., Christophe, A. & Mehler, J. (1993). Attentional allocation within the syllabic structure of spoken words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 373-389.
  411. Paulesu, E., Démonet, J.-F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S.-F., Cossu, G., Habib, M., Frith, C. & Frith, U. (2001). Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291, 2165-2167.
  412. Paulesu, E., Frith, U., Snowling, M., Gallagher, A., Morton, J., Frackowiak, R. & Frith, C. (1996). Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from PET scanning. Brain, 119, 143-157.
  413. Peereman, R., Lété, B. & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2007). Manulex-infra: distributional characteristics of grapheme-phoneme mappings, infralexical and lexical units in child-directed written material. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 39, 593-603.
  414. Perea, M. & Carreiras, M. (1998). Effects of syllable frequency and syllable neighbourhood frequency in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 134-144.
  415. Perea, M. & Carreiras, M. (2008). Do orthotactics and phonology constrain the transposed-letter effect? Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 69-92
  416. Peretz, I., Lussier, I. & Béland, R. (1998). The differential role of syllabic structure in stem completion for French and English. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 10, 75-112.
  417. Perfetti, C. (1989). Représentation et prise de conscience au cours de l’apprentissage de la lecture. In L. Rieben & C. Perfetti (Eds.), L’apprenti lecteur (pp. 61-82). Neuchâtel-Paris: Delachaux & Niestlé.
  418. Perfetti, C. (1997). Psycholinguistique de l’orthographe et de la lecture. In L. Rieben, M. Fayol & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Des orthographes et leur acquisition (pp. 37-56). Paris: Delachaux & Niestlé.
  419. Perfetti, C. (2003). The universal grammar of reading.Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 3-24.
  420. Perfetti, C., Bell, L. & Delaney, S. (1988). Automatic (prelexical) phonetic activation in silent word reading: evidence from backward masking. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 59-70.
  421. Perret, C., Bonin, P. & Méot, A. (2006). Syllable priming effects in picture naming in French: lost in the sea! Experimental Psychology, 53, 95-104.
  422. Perry, C. & Ziegler, J. (2004). Beyond the two-strategy model of skilled spelling: Effects of consistency, grain size, and orthographic redundancy. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 325-356.
  423. Perry, C., Ziegler, J. & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychological Review, 114, 273-315.
  424. Pitt, M., Smith, K. & Klein, J. (1998). Syllabic effects in word processing: evidence from the structural induction paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1596-1611.
  425. Plaut, D., McClelland, J., Seidenberg, M. & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56-115.
  426. Plaza, M. (2002). Troubles de l’accès au lexique dans les dyslexies de développement. In R. Cheminal & V. Brun (Eds.), Les dyslexies (pp. 49-55). Paris: Masson.
  427. Price, C. (2000). The anatomy of language: contributions from functional neuroimagery. Journal of Anatomy, 197, 335-359.
  428. Price, P. (1980). Sonority and syllabicity. Phonetica, 37, 327-343.
  429. Prinzmetal, W., Hoffman, H. & Vest, K. (1991). Automatic processes in word perception: an analysis from illusory conjunctions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 902-923.
  430. Prinzmetal, W., Treiman, R. & Rho, S. (1986). How to see a reading unit. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 461-475.
  431. Pugh, K., Mencl, W., Jenner, A., Katz, L., Frost, S., Lee, J., Shaywitz, S. & Shaywitz, B. (2000). Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia). Mental Retardation Development Disabilities Research Reviews, 6, 207-213.
  432. Pugh, K., Mencl, W., Jenner, A., Katz, L., Frost, S., Lee, J., Shaywitz, S. & Shaywitz, B. (2001). Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 479-492.
  433. Pulgram, E. (1970). Syllable, word, nexus, cursus. The Hague: Mouton.
  434. Quéné, H. (1992). Durational cues for word segmentation in Dutch. Journal of Phonetics, 20, 331-350.
  435. Quercia, P., Seigneuric, A., Chariot, S., Vernet, P., Pozzo, T., Bron, A., Creuzot-Garcher, C. & Robichon, F. (2005). Proprioception oculaire et dyslexie de développement. Journal Français d’Ophtalmologie, 28, 713-723.
  436. Radeau, M., Morais, J. & Seguí, J. (1995). Phonological priming between monosyllabic spoken words. Journal of experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1297-1311.
  437. Rae, C., Harasty, J., Dzendrowskyj, T., Talcott, J., Simpson, J. & Blamire, A. (2002). Neuropsychologia, 40, 1285-1292.
  438. Rae, C., Lee, M., Dixon, R., Blamire, A., Thompson, C., Styles, P., Talcott, J., Richardson, A. & Stein, J. (1998). Metabolic abnormalities in developmental dyslexia detected by 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Lancet, 351, 1849-1852.
  439. Ramus, F. (2001). Outstanding questions about phonological processing in dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7, 197-216.
  440. Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212-218.
  441. Ramus, F. (2004). Neurobiology of dyslexia: a reinterpretation of the data. Trends in Neurosciences, 27, 720-726.
  442. Ramus, F., Pidgeon, E. & Frith, U. (2003a). The relationship between motor control and phonology in dyslexic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 712-722.
  443. Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S., Day, B., Castellote, J., White, S. & Frith, U. (2003b). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841-865.
  444. Ramus, F. & Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 129-141.
  445. Rapp, B. (1992). The nature of sublexical orthographic organisation: the bigram through hypothesis examined. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 33-53.
  446. Rastle, K. & Coltheart, M. (1999). Serial and strategic effects in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 482-503.
  447. Rativeau, S., Zagar, D., Jourdain, C. & Colé, P. (1997). L’évaluation diagnostique de la lecture chez l’apprenti lecteur. In C. Miniac & B. Lété (Eds.), L’illetrisme: de la prevention aux stratégies de formation chez l’adulte (pp. 161-202). Bruxelles: DeBoeck.
  448. Rayner, K., Foorman, B., Perfetti, C., Pesetsky, D. & Seidenberg, M. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 31-74.
  449. Reed, M. (1989). Speech perception and the discrimination of brief auditory cues in dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 270-292.
  450. Rey, A., Jacobs, A., Schmidt-Weigand, F. & Ziegler, J. (1998). A phoneme effect in visual word recognition. Cognition, 68, 71-80.
  451. Rey, V., De Martino, S., Espesser, R. & Habib, M. (2002). Temporal processing and phonological impairment in dyslexia: effect of phoneme lengthening on order judgment of two consonants. Brain and Language, 80, 576-591.
  452. Rey, A. & Schiller, N. (2005). Graphemic complexity and multiple print-to-sound associations in visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 33, 76-85.
  453. Rey, A., Ziegler, J. & Jacobs, A. (2000). Graphemes are perceptual reading units. Cognition, 75, 1-12.
  454. Reynolds, M. & Besner, D. (2005). Basic processes in reading: a critical review of pseudohomophone effects in reading aloud and a new computational account. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 622-646.
  455. Robichon F., Bouchard P., Démonet J.-F., & Habib M. (2000). Developmental dyslexia: re-evaluation of the corpus callosum in male adults. European Neurology, 43, 233-237.
  456. Rocheron, I., Lorenzi, C., Füllgrabe, C., Legros, V. & Dumont, A. (2002). Temporal envelope perception in dyslexic children. NeuroReport, 13, 1683–1687.
  457. Rondal, J.-A. (1997). L’évaluation du langage. Bruxelles: Mardaga.
  458. Rosen, S. (2003). Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: is there a deficit? What is its nature? Does it explain anything? Journal of Phonetics, 31, 509-527.
  459. Rosen, S. & Manganari, E. (2001). Is there a relationship between speech and nonspeech auditory processing in children with dyslexia? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 720-736.
  460. Rouibah, A. & Taft, M. (2001). The role of syllabic structure in French visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 29, 373-381.
  461. Rubenstein, H., Lewis, S. & Rubesntein, M. (1971). Evidence for phonological coding in visual word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 645-657.
  462. Ruff, S., Boulanouar, K., Cardebat, D., Celsis, P. & Démonet, J.-F. (2001). Brain correlates of impaired categorical phonetic perception in adult dyslexics. NeuroImage, 13, S595.
  463. Ruff, S., Cardebat, D., Marie, N. & Démonet, J.-F. (2002). Enhanced response of the left frontal cortex to slowed down speech in dyslexia: an fMRI study. NeuroReport, 13, 1285-1289.
  464. Ruff, S., Marie, N., Celsis, P., Cardebat, D. & Démonet, J.-F. (2003). Neural substrates of impaired categorical perception of phonemes in adult dyslexics: an fMRI study. Brain and Language, 53, 331-334.
  465. Rumsey, J., Donohue, B., Brady, D., Nace, K., Giedd, J. & Andreason, P. (1997). A magnetic resonance imaging study of planum temporale asymmetry in men with developmental dyslexia. Archives of Neurology, 54, 1481-1489.
  466. Rumsey, J., Horwitz, B., Donohue, B., Nace, K., Maisog, J. & Andreason, P. (1999). A functional lesion in developmental dyslexia: left angular gyral blood flow predicts severity. Brain and Language, 70, 187-204.
  467. Russo, N., Nicol, T., Zecker, S., Hayes, E. & Kraus, N. (2005). Auditory training improves neural timing in the human brainstem. Behavioural Brain Research, 156, 95-103.
  468. Salmelin, R., Service, E., Kiesilä, P., Uutela, K. & Salonen, O. (1996). Impaired visual word processing in dyslexia revealed with magnetoencephalography. Annals of Neurology, 40, 157-162.
  469. Scarborough, H. (1998). Predicting the future achievement of second graders with reading disabilities: contributions of phonemic awareness, verbal memory, rapid serial naming, and IQ. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 115-136.
  470. Schiller, N. (1998). The effect of visually masked primes on the naming latencies of words and pictures. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 484-507.
  471. Schiller, N. (1999). Masked syllable priming of English nouns. Brain and Language, 68, 300-305.
  472. Schiller, N. (2000). Single word production in English: the role of subsyllabic units during speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 512-528.
  473. Schiller, N. (2004). The onset effect in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 477-490.
  474. Schiller, N. & Costa, A. (2006). Activation of segments, not syllables, during phonological encoding in speech production. The Mental Lexicon, 1, 231-250.
  475. Schiller, N., Costa, A. & Colomé, A. (2002). Phonological encoding of single words: in search of the lost syllable. In C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (Eds.), Laboratory Phonology 7 (pp. 35-59). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  476. Schiller, N., Meyer, A. & Levelt, W. (1997). The syllabic structure of spoken words: evidence from the syllabification of intervocalic consonants. Language and Speech, 40, 103-140.
  477. Schneider, W., Küspert, P., Roth, E., Visé, M. & Marx, H. (1997). Short- and long-term effects of training phonological awareness in kindergarten: evidence from two german studies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 311-340.
  478. Schulte-Körne, G., Bartling, J., Deimel, W. & Remschmidt, H. (1999). Attenuated hemispheric lateralization in dyslexia: evidence of a visual processing deficit. NeuroReport, 10, 3697-3701.
  479. Schulte-Körne, G., Bartling, J., Deimel, W. & Remschmidt, H. (2004). Visual evoked potentials elicited by coherently moving dots in dyslexic children. Neuroscience Letters, 357, 207-210.
  480. Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J. & Remschmidt, H. (1998a). Role of auditory temporal processing for reading and spelling disability. Perception and Motor Skills, 86, 1043-1047.
  481. Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J. & Remschmidt, H. (1998b). Auditory processing and dyslexia: evidence for a specific speech processing deficit. NeuroReport, 26, 337-340.
  482. Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J. & Remschmidt, H. (2001). Speech perception deficit in dyslexic adults as measured by mismatch negativity (MMN). International of Psychophysiology, 40, 77-87.
  483. Sears, C., Hino, Y. & Lupker, S. (1995). Neighborhood size and neighborhood frequency effects in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 876-900.
  484. Sebastián-Gallés, N., Dupoux, E., Seguí, J. & Mehler, J. (1992). Contrasting syllabic effects in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 18-32.
  485. Seguí, J. (1997). La perception du langage parlé: données et théories. In J. Lambert & J.-L. Nespoulos (Eds.), Perception auditive et compréhension du langage (pp. 15-23). Paris: Solal.
  486. Seguí, J., Dupoux, E. & Mehler, J. (1990). The role of the syllable in speech segmentation, phoneme identification and lexical access. In G. Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing (pp. 263-280). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  487. Seguí, J. & Ferrand, L. (2000). Leçons de parole. Odile Jacob: Paris.
  488. Seguí, J., Frauenfelder, U. & Hallé, P. (2001). Phonotactic constraints shape speech perception: implications for sublexical and lexical processing. In E. Dupoux (Ed.), Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler (pp. 195-208). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  489. Seidenberg, M. (1987). Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: access units or orthographic redundancy? In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: the psychology of reading (pp. 245-263). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  490. Seidenberg, M., & McClelland, J. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523-568.
  491. Seidenberg, M. & Plaut, D. (2006). Progress in understanding word reading: Data fitting versus theory building. In S. Andrews. (Ed.), From inkmarks to ideas: Current issues in lexical processing (pp. 1-29). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  492. Seidenberg, M., Plaut, D., Petersen, A., McClelland, J. & McRae, K. (1994). Nonword pronunciation and models of word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1177-1196.
  493. Selkirk, E. (1982). The syllable. In H. van der Hulst & N. Smith (Eds.), The structure of phonological representations (pp. 337-383). Dordrecht: Foris.
  494. Selkirk, E. (1984). On the major class features and syllable theory. In M. Arnolf & R. Octyle (Eds.), Language and sound structure (pp. 107-136). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  495. Sendlmeier, W. (1995). Feature, phoneme, syllable or word: how speech is mentally represented? Phonetica, 52, 131-143.
  496. Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Carré, R. & Démonet, J.-F. (2001). Perceptual categorization of speech sounds in dyslexics. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 384-399.
  497. Serniclaes, W., Van Heghe, S., Mousty, P., Carré, R. & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2004). Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 336-361.
  498. Sevald, C., Dell, G. & Cole, J. (1995). Syllable structure in speech production: are syllables chunks or schemas? Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 807-820.
  499. Seymour, P. (1990). Developmental dyslexia. In M. Eysenck (Ed.), Cognitive psychology: an international review (pp. 135-196). London: John Wiley & Sons.
  500. Seymour, P. (1997). Les fondations du développement orthographique et morphographique. In L. Rieben, M. Fayol & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Des orthographes et leur acquisition (pp. 385-403). Paris: Delachaux & Niestlé.
  501. Seymour, P., Aro, M. & Erskine, J. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143-174.
  502. Seymour, P. & Duncan, L. (1997). Small versus large unit theories of reading acquisition. Dyslexia, 3, 125-134.
  503. Seymour, P., Duncan, L. & Bolik, F. (1999). Rhymes and phonemes in the common unit task: replications and implications for beginning reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 22, 113-130.
  504. Shannon, R., Zeng, F., Kamath, V., Wygonski, J. & Ekelid, M. (1995). Speech recognition with primarily temporal cues. Science, 270, 303-304.
  505. Share, D. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151-218.
  506. Share, D. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: a direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1309-1324.
  507. Share, D., Jorm, A., MacLean, R. & Matthews, R. (2002). Temporal processing and reading disability. Reading and Wrting: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 151-178.
  508. Shaywitz, S. & Shaywitz, B. (2005). Dyslexia (Specific Reading Disability). Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1301-1309
  509. Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Pugh, K., Fulbright, R., Constable, R., Mencl, W., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A., Skudlarski, P., Fletcher, J., Katz, L., Marchione, K., Lacadie, C., Gatenby, C. & Gore, J. (1998). Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. PNAS, 95, 2636-2641.
  510. Shaywitz, B., Shaywitz, S., Pugh, K., Fulbright, R., Mencl, W., Constable, R., Skudlarski, P., Fletcher, J., Lyon, G. & Gore, J. (2001). The neurobiology of dyslexia. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 1, 291-299.
  511. Shaywitz, B., Shaywitz, S., Pugh, K., Mencl, W., Fulbright, R., Skudlarski, P., Constable, R., Marchione, K., Fletcher, J., Lyon, G. & Gore, J. (2002). Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 101-110.
  512. Silani, G., Frith, U., Démonet, J.-F., Fazio, F., Perani, D., Price, C., Frith, C. & Paulesu, E. (2005). Brain abnormalities underlying altered activation in dyslexia: a voxel based morphometry study. Brain, 128, 2453-2461.
  513. Simos, P., Breier, J., Fletcher, J., Bergman, E. & Papanicolaou, A. (2000). Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: a magnetic source imaging approach. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 809-816.
  514. Simos, P., Fletcher, J., Bergman, E., Breier, J., Foorman, B., Castillo, E., Davis, R., Fitzgerald, M. & Papanicolaou, A. (2002). Dyslexia-specific brain activation profile become normal following successful remedial training. Neurology, 58, 1203-13.
  515. Sireteanu, R., Goertz, R., Bachert, I. & Wandert, T. (2005). Children with developmental dyslexia show a left visual “minineglect”. Vision Research, 45, 3075-3082.
  516. Skottun, B. (2000). The magnocellular deficit theory of dyslexia: the evidence from contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 40, 111-127.
  517. Skottun, B. (2005). Magnocellular reading and dyslexia. Vision Research, 45, 133-134.
  518. Slaghuis, W. & Ryan J. (1999). Spatio-temporal contrast sensitivity, coherent motion, and visible persistence in developmental dyslexia. Vision Research, 39, 651-668.
  519. Slaghuis, W. & Ryan, J. (2003). Directional motion contrast sensitivity in developmental dyslexia. Vision, 46, 3291-3303.
  520. Slowiaczek, L., Soltano, E. & Bernstein, H. (2006). Lexical and metrical stress in word recognition: lexical or pre-lexical influences? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 491-512.
  521. Slowiaczek, L., Soltano, E., Wieting, S. & Bishop, K. (2003). An investigation of phonology and orthography in spoken-word recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 233-262.
  522. Snowling, M. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7, 37-46.
  523. Snowling, M., Gallagher, A. & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358-373.
  524. Snowling, M., Stackhouse, J. & Rack, J. (1986). Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia: a developmental analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 309-339.
  525. Spencer, A. (1996). Phonology. Oxford: Blackwell.
  526. Spencer, K. (2000). Is English a dyslexic language? Dyslexia, 6, 152-162.
  527. Spencer, K. (2007). Predicting children’s word-spelling difficulty for common English words from measures of orthographic transparency, phonemic and graphemic length and word frequency. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 305-338.
  528. Spencer, L. & Hanley, J. (2003). Effects of orthographic transparency on reading and phoneme awareness in children learning to read in Wales. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 1-28.
  529. Sperling, A., Lu, Z.-L., Manis, F. & Seidenberg, M. (2003). Selective magnocellular deficits in dyslexia: a “phantom contour” study. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1422-1429.
  530. Spinelli, E. & Ferrand, L. (2005). Psychologie du langage. L’écrit et le parlé, du signal à la signification. Paris: Armand Colin.
  531. Spinelli, E., McQueen, J. & Cutler, A. (2002). Resolution of liaison for lexical access in French. Revue française de linguistique appliquée, VII-1, 83-96.
  532. Spinelli, E. McQueen, J. & Cutler, A. (2003). Processing resyllabified words in French. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 233-254.
  533. Spinelli, E. & Radeau, M. (2004). La syllabe contraint-elle la cohorte initiale en français ? In L. Ferrand & J. Grainger (Eds.), Psycholinguistique Cognitive. Essais en l’honneur de Juan Segui (pp. 75-91). Bruxelles: DeBoeck.
  534. Spoehr, K. & Smith, E. (1973). The role of syllables in perceptual processing. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 71-89.
  535. Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Bonnet, P. (1996). New doubts on the importance of the logographic stage. Current Psychology of Cognition, 15, 173-208.
  536. Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Casalis, S. (1995). Reading and spelling acquisition in French first graders: longitudinal evidence. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 39-63.
  537. Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Casalis, S. (1996). Lire, lecture et écriture: acquisition et troubles du développement. Paris: PUF.
  538. Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Colé, P. (2003). Lecture et dyslexie. Approche cognitive. Paris: Dunod.
  539. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Lacert, P. & Serniclaes, W. (2000). On subtypes of developmental dyslexia: evidence from processing time and accuracy scores. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 88-104.
  540. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Lacert, P. & Colé, P. (1999). Déficits phonologiques et métaphonologiques chez des dyslexiques phonologiques et de surface. Réeducation Orthophonique, 197, 25-51.
  541. Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Siegel, L. (1997). A longitudinal study of the effects of syllabic structure on the development of reading and spelling skills in French. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 485-505.
  542. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. & Béchennec, D. (1998a). Phonological mediation and semantic and orthographic factors in silent reading in French. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 3-29.
  543. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L., Béchennec, D. & Serniclaes, W. (2003). Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading and in spelling: a four year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 194-217.
  544. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. & Bonnet, P. (1998b). Phonological mediation and orthographic factors in reading and spelling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 143-155.
  545. Stanovich, K., Siegel, L. & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 114-127.
  546. Stein, J. (2001). The magnocellular theory of developmental theory. Dyslexia, 7, 12-36.
  547. Stein, J. (2003). Visual motion sensitivity and reading. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1785-1793.
  548. Stein, J., Talcott, J. & Walsh, V. (2000). Controversy about the visual magnocellular deficit in developmental dyslexics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 209-211.
  549. Stein, J. & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 147-152.
  550. Stein, J. & Walsh, V. (1999). Impaired neural timing in developmental dyslexia – the magnocellular hypothesis. Dyslexia, 5, 59-77.
  551. Stein, J., Richardson, A. & Fowler, M. (2000). Monocular occlusion can improve binocular control and reading in dyslexics. Brain, 123, 164-170.
  552. Steinschneider, M., Volkov, I., Fishman, Y., Oya, H., Arezzo, J. & Howard, M. (2005). Intracortical responses in human and monkey primary auditory cortex support a temporal processing mechanism for encoding of the voice onset time phonetic parameter. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 170-186.
  553. Stenneken, P., Conrad, M. & Jacobs, A. (2007). Processing of syllables in production and recognition tasks. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 65-78.
  554. Stoodley, C., Harrison, E. & Stein, J. (2006). Implicit motor learning deficits in dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia, 44, 795-798.
  555. Stone, G., Vanhoy, M. & Van Orden, G. (1997). Perception is a two-way street: feedforward and feedback phonology in visual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 337-359.
  556. Studdert-Kennedy, M. (2002). Deficits in phoneme awareness does not arise from failures in rapid auditory processing. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 5-14.
  557. Suomi, K., McQueen, J. & Cutler, A. (1997). Vowel harmony and speech segmentation in Finnish. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 422-444.
  558. Swan, D. & Goswami, U. (1997). Picture naming and deficits in developmental dyslexia: the phonological representations hypothesis. Brain and Language, 56, 334-353.
  559. Szenkovits, G. & Ramus, F. (2005). Exploring dyslexics’ phonological deficit I: lexical vs. sub-lexical and input vs. output processes. Dyslexia, 11, 253-268.
  560. Tabossi, P., Collina, S., Mazetti, M. & Zoppello, M. (2000). Syllables in the processing of spoken Italian. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 758-775.
  561. Taft, M. (1979). Lexical access via an orthographic code: the Basic Orthographic Syllabic Structure (BOSS). Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 21-39.
  562. Taft, M. (1992). The body of the BOSS: subsyllabic units in the lexical processing of polysyllabic words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1004-1014.
  563. Taft, M. (2001). Processing of orthographic structure by adults of different reading ability. Language and Speech, 44, 351-376.
  564. Taft, M. (2002). Orthographic processing of polysyllabic words by native and nonnative English speakers. Brain and Language, 81, 532-544.
  565. Taft, M., Álvarez, C. & Carreiras, M. (2007). Cross-language differences in the use of internal orthographic structure when reading polysyllabic words. The Mental Lexicon, 2, 49-63.
  566. Taft, M. & Radeau, M. (1995). The influence of the phonological characteristics of a language on the functional units of reading: a study in French. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 330-346.
  567. Taipale, M., Kaminen, N., Nopola-Hemmi, J., Haltia, T., Myllyluoma, B., Lyytinen, H., Muller, K., Kaaranen, M., Lindsberg, P., Hannula-Jouppi, K. & Kere, J. (2003). A candidate gene for developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein dynamically regulated in brain. PNAS, 100, 11553-11558.
  568. Talcott, J., Hansen, P., Assoku, E. & Stein, J. (2000a). Visual motion sensitivity in dyslexia: evidence for temporal and motion energy integration deficits. Neuropsychologia, 38, 935-943.
  569. Talcott, J., Hansen, P., Willis-Owen, C., McKinnell, I., Richardson, A. & Stein, J. (1998). Visual magnocellular impairment in adult developmental dyslexics. Neuro-Ophtalmology, 20, 187-201.
  570. Talcott, J., Witton, C., McLean, M., Hansen, P., Rees, A., Green, G., & Stein, J.
    (1999). Can sensitivity to auditory frequency modulation predict children’s phonological and reading skills? NeuroReport, 10, 2045-2050.
  571. Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9, 182-198.
  572. Tallal, P., Miller, S., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W. & Merzenich, M. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81-84.
  573. Tallal, P. & Piercy, M. (1973). Developmental aphasia: Impaired rate of non-verbal processing as a function of sensory modality. Neuropsychologia, 11, 389--398.
  574. Tallal, P. & Piercy, M. (1975). Developmental aphasia: the perception of brief vowels and extended stop consonants. Neuropsychologia, 13, 69-74.
  575. Tanner, W. & Swets, J. (1954).  A decision-making theory of visual detection.  Psychological Review, 61, 401-409.
  576. Temple, E., Deutsch, G., Poldrack, R., Miller, S., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. & Gabrieli, J. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from fMRI. PNAS, 100, 2860-2865.
  577. Temple, Poldrack, Protopapas, Nagarajan, Salz, Tallal, Merzenich, Gabrieli, J. (2000). Disruption of the neural response to rapidly transient acoustic stimuli in dyslexia: evidence from fMRI. PNAS, 97, 13907-13912.
  578. Temple, E., Poldrack, R., Salidis, J., Deutsch, G., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. & Gabrieli, J. (2001) Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: an fMRI study. NeuroReport, 12, 299-308.
  579. Tesar, B. (1995). Computational Optimality Theory. Doctoral dissertation, University of Colorado at Boulder. ROA-90. http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/~tesar/papers/tesardis.pdf.
  580. Transler, C., Leybaert, J., Gombert, J.-É. (1999). Do deaf children use spoken syllables as reading units? Journal of Deaf Study and Deaf Education, 4, 124-143.
  581. Treiman, R. (1986). The division between onsets and rimes in English syllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 476-491.
  582. Treiman, R. (1989). The internal structure of the syllable. In G. Carlson & M. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Linguistic structure in language processing (pp. 27-52). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
  583. Treiman, R. (1992). The role of intrasyllabic units in learning to read and spell. In D. Sawyer & B. Fox (Eds.), Phonological awareness in reading. The evolution of current perspective (pp. 65-106). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  584. Treiman, R. (2006). Knowledge about letters as a foundation for reading and spelling. In R. Joshi & P. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 581-599). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
  585. Treiman, R., Broderick, V., Tincoff, R. & Rodriguez, K. (1998). Children’s phonological awareness: confusions between phonemes that differ only in voicing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 3-21.
  586. Treiman, R. & Cassar, M. (1997). Can children and adults focus on sound as opposed to spelling in a phoneme counting task? Developmental Psychology, 33, 771-780.
  587. Treiman, R. & Chafetz, J. (1987). Are there onset -and rime- like units in printed words ? In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII. The psychology of reading (pp. 281-298). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  588. Treiman, R. & Danis, C. (1988). Syllabification of intervocalic consonants. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 87-104.
  589. Treiman, R., Fowler, C., Gross, J., Berch, D. & Weatherspoon, S. (1995c). Syllable structure or word structure? Evidence for onset and rime units with disyllabic and trisyllabic stimuli. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 132-155.
  590. Treiman, R. & Kessler, B. (1995). In defense of an onset-rhyme syllable structure for English. Language and Speech, 38, 127-142.
  591. Treiman, R. & Kessler, B. (2007). Learning to read. In M. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 657-666). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  592. Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijelbac-Babic, R. & Richmond-Welty, E. (1995b). The special role of rimes in the description, use and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107-136.
  593. Treiman, R. & Zukowski, A. (1990). Toward an understanding of English
    syllabification. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 66–85.
  594. Treiman, R. & Zukowski, A. (1991). Levels of phonological awareness. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 67-83). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  595. Treiman, R. & Zukowski, A. (1996). Children’s sensitivity to syllables, onsets, rimes, and phonemes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 193-215.
  596. Treiman, R., Zukowski, A. & Richmond-Welty, E. (1995a). What happened to the “n” of sink? Children’s spellings of final consonant clusters. Cognition, 55, 1-38.
  597. Treisman, A. & Schmidt, H. (1982). Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 107-141.
  598. Tremblay, K. & Kraus, N. (2002). Auditory training induces asymmetrical changes in cortical neural activity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 564-572.
  599. Valdois, S. (1996a). A case of developmental surface dyslexia and dysgraphia. Brain and Cognition, 32, 229-231.
  600. Valdois, S. (1996b). Les dyslexies développementales. In S. Carbonnel, P. Gillet, M.-D. Martory & S. Valdois (Eds.), Approche cognitive des troubles de la lecture et de l’écriture chez l’enfant et l’adulte (pp. 137-151). Marseille: Solal.
  601. Valdois, S., Bosse, M.-L., Ans B., Zorman, M., Carbonnel, S., David, D. & Pellat, J. (2003). Phonological and visual processing deficits are dissociated in developmental dyslexia: evidence from two case studies. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 543-572.
  602. Valdois, S., Gérard, C., Vaneau, P. & Dugas, M. (1995). Peripheral developmental dyslexia: a visual attentional account? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 31-67.
  603. Van Den Bosch, A., Content, A., Daelemans, W. & De Gelder, B. (1994). Measuring the complexity of writing systems. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 1, 178-188.
  604. Van Ingelghem, M., Van Wieringen, A., Wouters, J., Vandenbussche, E., Onghena, P. & Ghesquière, P. (2001). Psychophysical evidence for a genral temporal processing deficit in children with dyslexia. NeuroReport, 12, 3603-3607.
  605. Van Ijzendoorn, M. & Bus, A. (1994). Meta-analytic confirmation of the non-word reading deficit in developmental dyslexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 29, 266-275.
  606. Van Reybroeck, M. (2003). Élaboration d’une batterie d’épreuves évaluant les compétences phonologiques. Mémoire de DEA en Sciences Psychologiques et de l’Éducation, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgique.
  607. Vellutino, F., Fletcher, J., Snowling, M. & Scanlon, D. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2-40.
  608. Vellutino, F. & Scanlon,D. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merril-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321-363.
  609. Vennemann, T. (1988). Preference laws for syllable structure and the explanation of sound change.Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
  610. Ventura, P., Kolinsky, R.., Brito-Mendes, C. & Morais, J. (2001). Mental representations of the syllable internal structure are influenced by orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 393-418.
  611. Ventura, P., Morais, J., Pattamadilok, C. & Kolinsky, R. (2004). The locus of the orthographic consistency effect in auditory word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 57-95.
  612. Véronis, J. (1986). Étude quantitative sur le système graphique et phono-graphique du français. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 6(5), 501-531.
  613. Veuillet, E., Magnan, A. & Écalle, J. (2004). Déficits auditifs perceptifs et capacités en lecture chez les enfants dyslexiques: effet d’un entraînement audio-visuel. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 14, 103-132.
  614. Veuillet, E., Magnan, A., Écalle, J., Thai-Van, H. & Collet, L. (2007). Auditory processing disorder in children with reading disabilities: effect of audiovisual training. Brain, 130, 2915-2928.
  615. Vidyasagar, T. (2004). Neural underpinnings of dyslexia as a disorder of visuo-spatial attention. Clinical and Experimental Optometry, 87, 4-10.
  616. Visser, T., Boden, C. & Giaschi, D. (2004). Children with dyslexia: evidence for visual attention deficits in perception of rapid sequences of objects. Vision Research, 44, 2521-2535.
  617. Vitevitch, M. & Luce, P. (2005). Increases in phonotactic probability facilitate spoken nonword repetition. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 193-204.
  618. Vroomen, J. & De Gelder, B. (1995). Metrical segmentation and lexical inhibition in spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 98-108.
  619. Vroomen, J. & De Gelder, B. (1997). The activation of embedded words in spoken word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 710-720.
  620. Vroomen, J. & De Gelder, B. (1999). Lexical access of resyllabified words: evidence from phoneme monitoring. Memory & Cognition, 27, 413-421.
  621. Vroomen, J., Tuomainen, J. & De Gelder, B. (1998). The roles of word stress and vowel harmony in speech segmentation. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 133-149.
  622. Wagner, R. & Torgesen, J. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skill. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212.
  623. Wang, Y., Paramasivam, M., Thomas, A., Bai, J., Kaminen-Ahola, N., Kerer, J., Voskuil, J., Rosen, G., Galaburda, A., LoTurco, J., 2006). DYX1C1 functions in neural migration in developing neocortex. Neuroscience, 143, 512-522.
  624. Weekes, B. (1997). Differential effects of number of letters on words and nonword naming latency. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 439-456.
  625. Werker, J. & Tees, R. (1987). Speech perception in severely disabled and average reading children. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 48-61.
  626. White, S., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Hansen, P., Swettenham, J., Frith, U. & Ramus, F. (2006). The role of sensorimotor impairments in dyslexia: a multiple case study of dyslexic children, Developmental Science, 9, 237-255.
  627. Wible, B., Nicol, T. & Kraus, N. (2005). Correlation between brainstem and cortical auditory processes in normal and language-impaired children. Brain, 128, 417-423.
  628. Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1-33.
  629. Wimmer, H. & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development. Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51, 91-103.
  630. Wimmer, H. & Hummer, P. (1990). How German speaking first graders read and spell: doubts on the importance of the logographic stage. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 349-368.
  631. Wimmer, H., Landerl, K., Linortner, R. & Hummer, P. (1991). The relationship of phonemic awareness to reading acquisition: more consequences than precondition, but still important. Cognition, 40, 219-249.
  632. Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H. & Landerl, K. (1998). Poor reading: a deficit in skill-automatization or a phonological deficit? Scientific Studies of Reading, 2 , 321-340.
  633. Wise, B., Ring, J. & Olson, R. (2000). Individual differences in gains from computerassisted remedial reading with more emphasis on phonological analysis or accurate reading in context. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 197-235.
  634. Wioland, F. (1985). Les structures syllabiques du français. Fréquence et distribution des phonèmes consonantiques. Contraintes idiomatiques dans les séquences consonantiques. Genève: Slatkine-Champion.
  635. Witton, C., Stein, J., Stoodley, C., Rosner, B. & Talcott, J. (2002). Separate influences of acoustic AM and FM sensitivity on the phonological decoding skills of impaired and normal readers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 866-874.
  636. Witton, C., Talcott, J., Hansen, P., Richardson, A., Griffiths, T., Rees, A., Stein, J. & Green, G. (1998). Sensitivity to dynamic auditory and visual stimuli predicts nonword reading ability in both dyslexic and normal readers. Current Biology, 8, 791-797.
  637. Wright, B., Lombardino, L., King, W., Puranik, C., Leonard, C. & Merzenich, M. (1997). Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language-impaired children. Nature, 387, 176-178.
  638. Yates, M., Locker, L. & Simpson, G. (2004). The influence of phonological neighborhood on visual word perception. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , 11, 452-457.
  639. Zec, D. (1995). Sonority constraints on syllable structure. Phonology, 12, 85-129.
  640. Zeffiro, T. & Eden, G. (2001). The cerebellum and dyslexia: perpetrator or innocent bystander? Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 512-513.
  641. Zevin, J. & Seidenberg, M. (2002). Age of acquisition effects in reading and other tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 1-29.
  642. Ziegler, J., Castel, C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., Alario, F.-X. & Perry, C. (2008). Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: simulating individual differences and subtypes. Cognition , 107, 151-178.
  643. Ziegler, J., Ferrand, L. & Montant, M. (2004). Visual phonology: the effects of orthographic consistency on different auditory word recognition tasks. Memory & Cognition, 32 , 732-741.
  644. Ziegler, J., Ferrand, L., Jacobs, A., Rey, A. & Grainger, J. (2000). Visual and phonological codes in letter and word recognition: Evidence from incremental priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 53A , 671-692.
  645. Ziegler, J. & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3-29.
  646. Ziegler, J. & Goswami, U. (2006). Becoming literate in different languages: similar problems, different solutions. Developmental Science, 9, 429-453.
  647. Ziegler, J., Jacobs, A. & Klüppel, D. (2001). Pseudohomophone effects in lexical decision: still a challenge for current word recognition models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Performance and Perception, 27, 547-559.
  648. Ziegler, J., Jacobs, A. & Stone, G. (1996). Statistical analysis of the bidirectional inconsistency of spelling and sound in French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 28, 504-515.
  649. Ziegler, J., Montant, M. & Jacobs, A. (1997). The feedback consistency effect in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 37 , 533-554.
  650. Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C. & Coltheart, M. (2000). The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: an extension to German. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12 , 413-430.
  651. Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C. & Coltheart, M. (2003). Speed of lexical and nonlexical processing in French: the case of the regularity effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10 , 947-953.
  652. Ziegler, J., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D. & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169-193.
  653. Ziegler, J., Stone, G. & Jacobs, A. (1997). What is the pronunciation for –ough and the spelling for /u/? A database for computing feedforward and feedback consistency in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 29, 600-618.
  654. Ziegler, J., Van Orden, G. & Jacobs, A. (1997). Phonology can help or hurt the perception of print. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 23, 845-860.
  655. Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Judica, A., Orlandi, M. & Spinelli, D. (1999). Marker of developmental surface dyslexia in a language (Italian) with high grapheme-phoneme correspondence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 191-216.
  656. Zorman, M. (1999). Évaluation de la conscience phonologique et entraînement des capacities phonologiques en grande section de maternelle. Rééducation Orthophonique, 197, 139-157.
  657. Zorzi, M., Houghton, G. & Butterworth, B. (1998). Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1131-1161.
  658. Zwitserlood, P., Schriefers, H, Lahiri, A. & Van Donselaar, W. (1993). The role of syllables in the perception of spoken Dutch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 260-271.