References

Bharucha, J.J. (1984). Anchoring effects in music: the resolution of dissonance. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 485-518.

Bharucha, J.J. (1987). Music cognition and perceptual facilitation: A connectionist framework. Music Perception, 5, 1-30.

Bharucha, J. J., & Stoeckig, K. (1986). Reaction time and musical expectancy: Priming of chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12(4), 403-410.

Bharucha, J.J., & Stoeckig, K. (1987). Priming of chords: Spreading activation or overlapping frequency spectra? Perception & Psychophysics, 41(6), 519-524.

Bigand, E. (1997). Perceiving musical stability: The effect of tonal structure, rhythm and musical expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 808-822.

Bigand, E., Madurell, F., Tillmann, B., & Pineau, M. (1999). Effect of global structure and temporal organization on chord processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 184-197.

Bigand, E., Parncutt, R., & Lerdahl, F. (1996). Perception of musical tension in short chord sequences: the influences of harmonic function, sensory dissonance, horizontal motion, and musical training. Perception & Psychophysics, 58(1), 125-41.

Bigand, E., & Pineau, M. (1997). Global context effects on musical expectancy. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(7), 1098-1107.

Bigand, E., & Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2006). Are we “experienced listeners”? A review of the musical capacities that do not depend on formal musical training. Cognition, in press.

Bigand, E., Poulin, B., Tillmann, B., Madurell, F., & D'Adamo, D. A. (2003). Sensory versus cognitive components in harmonic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(1), 159-171.

Bigand, E., Tillmann, B., Poulin, B., D’Adamo, D.A., & Madurell, F. (2001). The effect of harmonic context on phoneme monitoring in vocal music. Cognition, 81, 11-20.

Bigand, E., Tillmann, B., & Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2006). A module for syntactic processing in music. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 195-196.

Bigand, E., Tillmann, B., Poulin-Charronnat, B., Manderlier, D. (2005). Repetition priming: Is music special? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A(8), 1347-1375.

Boltz, M. (1989a). Perceiving the end: effects of tonal relationships on melodic completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 749-761.

Boltz, M. G., & Jones, M. R. (1986). Does rule recursion make melodies easier to reproduce?

If not, what does? Cognitive Psychology, 18, 389-431.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). New York:

Academic Press.

Cohen, J. D., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J. (1993). PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 25(2), 257-271, http://psy.ck.sissa.it/.

Cuddy, L.L., & Lunney, C.A. (1995). Expectancies generated by melodic intervals:

Perceptual judgments of melodic continuity. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 451-462.

Cutting, J.E., Bruno, N., Brady, N.P., & Moore, C. (1992). Selectivity, scope, and simplicity of models: a lesson from fitting judgments of perceived depth, Journal of Experimental Psychology : General, 121(3), 364-381.

Dowling, W. J., & Harwood, D. L. (1986). Music Cognition. Orlando, Florida: Academic Press.

Francès, R. (1958). La perception de la musique. Paris: Vrin, Transl. J.W., Dowling (1988): The Perception of Music. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hébert, S., Peretz, I., & Gagnon, L. (1995). Perceiving the tonal ending of tune excerpts: The roles of pre-existing representation and musical expertise. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 193-209.

Holleran, S., Jones, M. R., & Butler, D. (1995). Perceiving implied harmony: The influence of melodic and harmonic context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 737-753.

Huron, D. (2006). Sweet Anticipation: Music and the Psychology of Expectation. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

Huron, D., & Parncutt, R. (1993). An improved model of tonality perception incorporating pitch salience and echoic memory. Psychomusicology, 12(2), 154-171.

Hutchins, S., & Palmer, C. (2008). Repetition priming in music. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(3), 693-707.

Justus, T.C., & Bharucha, J.J. (2001). Modularity in musical processing: the automaticity of harmonic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 1000-1011.

Krumhansl, C.L. (1979). The Psychological Representation of Musical Pitch in a Tonal Context. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 346-374.

Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. New York: Oxford University Press.

Krumhansl, C.L. (1995). Effects of musical context on similarity and expectancy. Systematische Musikwisssenschaft, 3, 211-250.

Krumhansl, C.L. (2004). The Cognition of Tonality – as We Know it Today. Journal of New Music Research, 33(3), 253-268.

Krumhansl, C. L., & Kessler, E. J. (1982). Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. Psychological Review, 89(4), 334-368.

Krumhansl, C.L., & Shepard, R.N. (1979). Quantification of the hierarchy of tonal functions within a diatonic context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 579-594.

Leman, M. (2000). An auditory model of the role of short-term memory in probe tone rating. Music Perception, 17, 437-460.

Narmour, E. (1990). The Analysis and Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures: The Implication-Realization Model. Chicago Press, 1990.

Parncutt, R. (1989). Harmony: a psychoacoustical approach. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1989.

Parncutt, R. (1994). Template-Matching Models of Musical Pitch and Rhythm Perception. Journal of New Music Research, 23, 145-167

Patel, A.D. (2003). Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 674-681.

Pearce, M.T., & Wiggins, G.A. (2006). Expectation in melody: the influence of context and learning. Music Perception, 23(5), 377-405.

Prentice, D.A., & Miller, D.T. (1992). When small effects are impressive. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 160-164.

Schellenberg, E.G., Adachi, M., Purdy, K.T., & McKinnon, M.C. (2002). Expectancy in Melody: Tests of Children and Adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(4), 511-537.

Schmuckler, M. A. (1989). Expectation in music: Investigation of melodic and harmonic processes. Music Perception, 7(2), 109-149.

Tekman, H.G., & Bharucha, J.J. (1992). Time course of chord priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 51(1), 33-39.

Tekman, H.G., & Bharucha, J.J. (1998). Implicit knowledge versus psychoacoustic similarity in priming of chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(1), 252-260.

Terhardt, E. (1974). Pitch, consonance, and harmony. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 55(5), 1061-1069.

Terhardt, E., Stoll, G., & Seewann, M. (1982). Pitch of complex signals according to virtual-pitch theory: test, examples, and predictions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 71(3), 671-678.

Tillmann, B. (2005). Implicit investigations of tonal knowledge in non-musician listeners. In G. Avanzini, L. Lopez, S. Koelsch, & M. Majno (Eds.), The neurosciences and music II: From perception to performance (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 1060, pp. 100-110). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

Tillmann, B., Bharucha, J. J., & Bigand, E. (2000). Implicit learning of tonality: A self-organizing approach. Psychological Review, 107(4), 885-913.

Tillmann, B., & Bigand, E. (2001). Global context effect in normal and scrambled musical sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(5), 1185-1196.

Tillmann, B., & Bigand, E. (2004). Musical priming: schematic expectations resist repetition priming. Proceedings of the 8 th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition, Evanston, IL.

Tillmann, B., Bigand, E., Escoffier, N., & Lalitte, P. (2006). The influence of musical relatedness on timbre discrimination. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 343-358.

Tillmann, B., Janata, P., Birk, J., & Bharucha, J.J. (2003). The costs and benefits of tonal centers for chord processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 470-482.

Tillmann, B., Peretz, I., Bigand, E., & Gosselin, N. (2007). Harmonic priming in an amusic patient: The power of implicit tasks. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 603-622.

Van Immerseel, L., & Martens, J. (1992). Pitch and voiced/unvoiced determination with an auditory model. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91, 3511-3526.