Références Bibliographiques

  1. ADOLPHS R. L., CAHILL R., SCHUL R. & BABINSKY R. (1997). Impaired declarative memory for emotional stimuli following bilateral amygdala damage in humans. Learning Memory, 4, 291-300.
  2. ANDERSEN R., SNYDER L.H., BRADLEY D.C. & XING J. (1997). Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 20, 303–330.
  3. ANDERSON A. & PHELPS E. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411, 305-309.
  4. ANSERMET F. & MAGISTRETTI P. (2004). A chacun son cerveau. Plasticité neuronale et inconscient, Paris, Odile Jacob, 264 p.
  5. ANSERMET F. & MAGISTRETTI P. (2006). L’Inconscient au crible des neurosciences. Dans Freud et la science, La Recherche, mai, 397, 36- 39.
  6. ARMSTRONG D.F., STOKOE W. C. & WILCOX S. E. (1995). Gesture and the nature of language, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  7. ASSOUN P.-L. (1981). Introduction à l’épistémologie freudienne, Paris, Edition Payot.
  8. ATCHLEY R.A., KEENEY M. & BURGESS C. (1999). Cerebral hemispheric mechanisms linking ambiguous word meaning retrieval and creativity. Brain and Cognition, 40, 479-499.
  9. BADDELEY A.D. (1986). Working Memory, Oxford, Oxford University Press.
  10. BADDELEY A.D. & HITCH G. (1974). Working Memory. Dans The psychology of learning and motivation, dir. G.H. Bower, 8, New York, Academic Press, 47-90.
  11. BALIBAR F. (2002). Le scientisme, Lacan, Freud et Le Dantec, Revue Alliage, 52.
  12. BAZAN A. (2001). Psychoanalyse en neurowetenschappen: de vraag naar een natuurwetenschappelijk kader voor een psychoanalytische taaltheorie. [Psychanalyse et neurosciences: la question d’un cadre scientifique pour une théorie psychanalytique du langage.] Dissertation non publiée, Université de Gand, Belgique.
  13. BAZAN A. (2002). The unconscious is affect sticking to phonology. Considerations on the role of articulation. Psychoanalytische Perspectieven, 20, 579-590.
  14. BAZAN A. (2005). La forme du langage en clinique. Une perspective neuropsychanalytique, Psychologie clinique, 18, 51-97.
  15. BAZAN A. (2006). Primary process language. Neuro-Psychoanalysis, 8, 157-159.
  16. BAZAN A. (2007). An attempt towards an integrative comparison of psychoanalytical and sensorimotor control theories of action. Dans Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition, dir. P. Haggard, Y. Rossetti et M. Kawato, New York, Oxford University Press, 319-338.
  17. BAZAN A. (2007). Des fantômes dans la voix. Une hypothèse neuropsychanalytique sur la structure de l’inconscient, Montréal, Editions Liber, Collection Voix Psychanalytiques.
  18. BAZAN A. (2008). A mind for resolving the interior-exterior distinctions. Dans Simulating the mind. the mental apparatus - a technical neuropsychoanalytical approach. (engeneering and neuro-psychoanalysis forum book), ed. D. Dietrich, G. Fodor, G. Zucker et D. Bruckner, Wien,Springer, 394-399.
  19. BAZAN A. & DE GRAVE D. (2003). Phonemes as organizing strange attractors, inédit.
  20. BAZAN A., GEERARDYN F., KNOCKAERT V., VAN BUNDER D. & VAN DE VIJVER G. (2002a). Language as the source of human unconscious processes. Evolution and Cognition, 8(2), 164-171.
  21. BAZAN A., GEERARDYN F., KNOCKAERT V., VAN BUNDER D. & VAN DE VIJVER G. (2002b). Anticipation as exercising (language) motor programs during dreams. A neuropsychoanalytical hypothesis, dans International Journal of Computing Anticipatory Systems, éds D. & M. Dubois, Liège,Chaos,Vol. 12, 181-194.
  22. BAZAN A., GEERARDYN F., KNOCKAERT V., VAN BUNDER D. & VAN DE VIJVER G. (2002). Language as the source of human unconscious processes. Evolution and Cognition, 8, 1-8.
  23. BAZAN A. & VAN BUNDER D. (2005). Some comments on the emotional and motor dynamics of language embodiment. A neurophysiological understanding of the freudian unconscious. Dans Body image & body schema, interdisciplinary perspectives, dir. H. De Preester et V. Knockaert, Amsterdam et Philadelphia, John Benjamins, 49-107.
  24. BAZAN A. & VAN DE VIJVER G. (in press). L’objet d’une science neuro-psychanalytique. Questions épistémologiques et mise à l’épreuve, dans Vers une neuropsychanalyse?, éds. L. Ouss, B. Golse, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  25. BAZAN A. & VAN DE VIJVER G. (in press). La constitution de la distinction entre intérieur et extérieur: proposition de recoupement entre Freud et les neurosciences modernes, dans Neurosciences et psychothérapie. réd. J. Monzee, Liber, Montréal.
  26. BAZAN A., VAN DRAEGE K., DE KOCK L., BRAKEL L.A.W., GEERARDYN F. & SHEVRIN H. (submitted). Empirical evidence for primary process mentation in acute psychosis.
  27. BAZAN A., WINER S.E., SHEVRIN H., SNODGRASS M. & BRAKEL L.A.W. (2005). Unconscious primary process language: an ERP study, présenté à la Journée de Recherche de la 6ième Conférence Internationale de Neuro-psychanalyse, Rio de Janeiro, Brésil, 27 juillet 2005.
  28. BEEMAN M. (1998). Coarse semantic coding and discourse comprehension. Dans Right hemisphere language comprehension: perspectives from cognitive neuroscience, dir. M. Beeman et C. Chiarello, Mahwah (NJ), 255-284.
  29. BEEMAN M.J. & BOWDEN E. (2000). The right hemisphere maintains solution-related activation for yet-to-be solved insight problems. Memory & Cognition, 28, 1231-1241.
  30. BERNAT E., BUNCE S. & SHEVRIN H. (2001). Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 11-34.
  31. BERNFELD S. (1951). Sigmund Freud, M.D., 1882-1885. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 32, 204-216.
  32. BICK P.A. & KINSBOURNE M. (1987). Auditory hallucinations and subvocal speech in schizophrenic patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 222-225.
  33. BILDER R.M. & LEFEVER F.F., eds. (1998). Neuroscience of the mind on the centennial of Freud’s Project for a scientific psychology. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 843.
  34. BISIACH E. & GEMINANI G. (1991). Anosognosia related to hemiplegia and hemianopia. Dans Awareness of deficit after brain injury, dir. G.P. Prigatano et D.L. Schacter, Oxford, Oxford University Press, 17-39.
  35. BLAKEMORE S.J. & SIRIGU A. (2003). Action prediction in the cerebellum and in the parietal lobe. Experimental Brain Research, 153, 239-245.
  36. BLAKEMORE S.J., FRITH C.D. & WOLPERT D.M. (1999). Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 5, 551–559.
  37. BLAKEMORE S.J., GOODBODY S.J. & WOLPERT D.M. (1998). Predicting the consequences of our own actions: the role of sensorimotor context estimation. The Journal of neuroscience, 18, 18, 7511-7518.
  38. BLAKEMORE S.J., WOLPERT D.M. & FRITH C.D. (2004).Why can't you tickle yourself ?. Neuroreport, 11, 11, R11 - R16.
  39. BLEULER E. (1911). Dementia praecox oder die Gruppe der Schizophrenien, Leipzig, Deuticke.
  40. BOISSEL A. (2008). Psychopathologie d'un traumatisme crânien survenu durant l'enfance. Effets à long terme sur le sujet et sa famille. Directrice de recherche: Professeure Dominique Cupa, Université Paris Ouest-Nanterre-La Défense.
  41. BOISSEL A. (2009). Parcours scolaire après un traumatisme crânien: à propos d'un cas clinique. Nouvelle Revue de l'Adaptation et de la Scolarisation, numéro Avril-Juin 2009.
  42. BOOKHEIMER S.Y., ZEFFIRO T.A., BLAXTON T., GAILLARD W. & THEODORE W. (1995). Regional cerebral blood flow during object naming and word reading. Human Brain Mapping, 3, 93-106.
  43. BRAKEL L.A.W. (2009). Philosophy, psychoanalysis and the a-rational mind. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
  44. BRAKEL L.A.W. (in press). Unconscious Knowing and Other Essays in Psycho-Philosophical Analysis.Oxford, Oxford University Press.
  45. BRAKEL L.A.W. & SHEVRIN H. (2005). Anxiety, attributional thinking and primary process. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 86, 1-14.
  46. BRAKEL L.A.W., KLEINSORGE S., SNODGRASS M. & SHEVRIN H. (2000). The primary process and the unconscious: experimental evidence supporting two psychoanalytic presuppositions. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 81, 553–569.
  47. BRAKEL L.A.W., SHEVRIN H. & VILLA K.K. (2002). The priority of primary process categorizing: experimental evidence supporting a psychoanalytic developmental hypothesis. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 50, 483–505.
  48. BRAUN A., BALKIN T.J., WESENTEN N.J., CARSON R.E., VARGA M., BALDWIN P., SELBIE S., BELENKY G. &HERSCOVITCH P. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle, Brain, 120, 1173-1197.
  49. BRAUN A., BALKIN T.J., WESENTEN N.J., GWADRY F., CARSON R.E., VARGA M., BALDWIN P., BELENKY G. &HERSCOVITCH P. (1998). Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye movement sleep. Science, 279, 91-95.
  50. BRAUN A.R., BALKIN T.J., WESENSTEN N.J., GWADRY F., CARSON R.E., VARGA M., BALDWIN P., BELENKY G. & HERSCOVITCH P. (1998). Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye movement sleep. Science, 279, 91-95.
  51. BROWNELL H. (2000). Right hemisphere contributions to understanding lexical connotation and metaphor. Dans Language and the brain: representation and processing, dir. Y. Grodzinsky et L. Shapiro, San Diego (CA), Academic Press, 185-201.
  52. BUCCI W. (1997). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science. A multiple code theory, New York, Guilford Press.
  53. BURGESS C. & LUND K. (1998). Modeling cerebral asymmetries in high-dimensional space. Dans Right hemisphere language comprehension: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience, dir. M. Beeman et C. Chiarello, Mahwah, Erlbaum, 215-244.
  54. BURGESS C. & SIMPSON G.B. (1988). Cerebral hemispheric mechanisms in the retrieval of ambiguous word meanings. Brain and Language, 33, 86-103.
  55. BURTON M. W. (2001). The role of inferior frontal cortex in phonological processing. Cognitive Science, 25, 695-709.
  56. CAHILL L., BABINSKY R., MARKOWITSCH H. & MCCAUGH J. L. (1995). The amygdala and emotional memory. Nature, 377, 295-296.
  57. CAINE T.M. & HOPE K. (1967). Manual of Hysteroid-Obsessoid Questionnaire (HOQ). London, London University Press.
  58. CALLAN D., JONES J., MUNHALL K., KROOS C., CALLAN A. & VATIKIOTIS-BATESON E. (2002). Mirror neuron system activity and audiovisual speech perception, présenté à la huitième conférence internationale sur la description du fonctionnement du cerveau humain «Functional Mapping of the Human Brain», Sendai, Japon. (NeuroImage, 16, 2, 2002).
  59. CALLAN D.E., JONES J.A., MUNHALL K., CALLAN A.M., KROOS C. & VATIKIOTIS-BATESON E. (2003). Neural processes underlying perceptual enhancement by visual speech gestures. NeuroReport, 14, 2213–2218.
  60. CANESTRI J., DREHER A.U., LEUZINGER-BOHLEBER M., eds. (2003). Pluralism and unity? Methods of research in psychoanalysis, Karnac International Psychoanalysis Library.
  61. CARAMAZZA A. (1996). The brain’s dictionary. Nature, 380, 485-486.
  62. CARAMAZZA A. & HILLIS A.E. (1991). Lexical organization of nouns and verbs in the brain. Nature, 349, 788-790.
  63. CHANGEUX J.P. (1983). L'homme neuronal, Fayard, Paris.
  64. CHANGEUX J.-P. & CONNES A. (2000). Matière à penser, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  65. CHAPEROT C., CELACU V. & PISANI C. (2005). Réflexions à propos des thèmes et des propositions de Kandel relatives aux liens possibles entre psychanalyse des neurosciences pour la défense d’une irréductibilité de l’objet. L' Évolution Psychia trique,70, 131.
  66. CHEE M.W., SRIRAM N., SOON C. S. & LEE K.M. (2000). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the implicit association of concepts and attributes. Neuroreport, 11, 135-140.
  67. CHIARELLO C. (1985). Hemispheric dynamics in lexical access: automatic and controlled priming. Brain and Language, 26, 146-172.
  68. CHIARELLO C. (1992). On codes of meaning and the meaning of codes: semantic access and retrieval within and between hemispheres. Dans Right hemisphere language comprehension: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience, dir. M. Beeman et C. Chiarello, Mahwah, Erlbaum, 141-160.
  69. CHIARELLO C., RICHARDS L. & POLLOCK A. (1992). Semantic additivity and semantic inhibition: dissociable processes in the cerebral hemispheres? Brain and Language, 42, 52-76.
  70. CHRISTOFFELS I.K., FORMISANO E. & SCHILLER N.O., Neural correlates of verbal feedback processing: An fMRI study employing overt speech. Human Brain Mapping, in press.
  71. CLEGHORN J.M. & al. (1992). Towards a brain map of auditory hallucinations. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 1062-1069.
  72. COLANGELO A. & BUCHANAN L. (2005). Semantic ambiguity and the failure of inhibition hypothesis as an explanation for reading errors in deep dyslexia. Brain and Cognition, 57, 39-42.
  73. COLE J. D. & PAILLARD J. (1996). Living without touch and peripheral information about body position and movement: studies upon deafferented subjects. Dans The Body and the Self, dir. A.M. Bermudez et N. Eilan, Cambridge, mit-Bradford Press, 245-266.
  74. Collectif, Quartier Lacan, Témoignages sur Jacques Lacan, L'Espace Analytique, Denoël, 2001.
  75. CORBALLIS M.C. (1999). The gestural origins of language. American Scientist, 87, 138-145.
  76. CRAIG A.D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 655-666.
  77. CRAMMOND D.J. (1997). Motor imagery: never in your wildest dream. Trends in Neurosciences, 20, 2, 54-57.
  78. CROWNE D. & MARLOWE D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349-354.
  79. CUTLER A. & CLIFTON C. (1999). Comprehending spoken language: a blueprint of the listener. Dans The Neurocognition of Language, dir. C.M. Brown et P. Hagoort Oxford, Oxford University Press, 123-166.
  80. CUTLER A., DEMUTH K. & MCQUEEN J.M. (2002). Universality versus language-specificity in listening to running speech. Psychological Science, 13, 3, 258-262.
  81. DAMASIO A.R. (1994). Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain, New York, Putnam.
  82. DAMASIO A.R. (1999). The feeling of what happens. Body, emotion and the making of consciousness, Londres, Random House.
  83. DAMASIO A.R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza. Joy, sorrow and the feeling brain, Londres, Random House.
  84. DAMASIO A.R., GRABOWSKI T.J., BECHARA A., DAMASIO H., PONTO L.L.B., PARVIZI J., & HICHWA R. D(1998). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1049-1056.
  85. DAMASIO H., GRABOWSKI T.J., TRANEL D., HICHWA R.D. & DAMASIO A.R. (1996). A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature, 380, 499-505.
  86. DANIEL M.-P., MORES C., CARITE L., BOYER P. & MICHEL D. (2006). Dysfunctions of spatial cognition: the case of schizophrenic patients. Cognitive Processing, 7, Suppl. 1, S173.
  87. DAVID A.S. (1994). The neuropsychological origin of auditory hallucinations. Dans The neuropsychology of schizophrenia, dir. A. S. David et J. C. Cutting, Hove, Lawrence Erlbaum, 269-313.
  88. DAVIS B. & MACNEILAGE P. (1995). The articulatory basis of babbling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 1199-1211.
  89. DE HOUWER J. & EELEN P. (1998). An affective variant of the simon paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 45-61.
  90. DE PREESTER H. (2007). The deep bodily origins of the subjective perspective: models and their problems. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 3, 604-618.
  91. DE RENZI E. & DI PELLEGRINO G. (1995). Sparing of verbs and preserved but ineffectual reading in patient with impaired word production. Cortex, 31, 619-636.
  92. DEACON T. (1997). The symbolic species. The co-evolution of language and the human brain, Londres, Penguin Press, 69-83.
  93. DEANE P.D. (1993). Multimodal spatial representation: on the semantic unity of over and other polysemous prepositions, Duisburg, LAUD.
  94. DECETY J. (1996). Neural representations for action. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 7, 285-297.
  95. DECETY J. & GRÈZES J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 172-178.
  96. DECETY J., PERANI D., JEANNEROD M., BETTINARDI V., TADARY B., WOODS R., MAZZIOTTA J.C. & FAZIO F. (1994). Mapping motor representations with positron emission tomography. Nature, 371, 600-602.
  97. DECLERQ F. (1995). De paranoïde logica van de betekenis: psychoanalyse en communicatie. Psychoanalytische Perspektieven, 27, 87-101.
  98. DEHAENE S. , NACCACHE L., LECLEC ' H G. , KOECHLIN E. , MUELLER M. , DEHAENE - LAMBERTZ G. , VAN DE MOORTELE P.F. & LE BIHAN D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597–600.
  99. DEMONET J.F., WISE R. & FRACKOWIAK R.S.J. (1993). Language functions explored in normal subjects by positron emission tomography: a critical review. Human Brain Mapping, 1, 44.
  100. DEMOULINC. (2001). Enjeux de la theorie lacanienne. Psychoanalytische Perspectieven, 46, 7-18.
  101. DESCARTES R. (1641/2004). Méditations métaphysiques, trad. F. Khodoss, Paris, PUF.
  102. DESIMONE R. & DUNCAN J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193-222.
  103. DESMURGET M., EPSTEIN C.M., TURNER R.S., PRABLANC C., ALEXANDER G.E., & GRAFTON S.T. (1999). Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 563-567.
  104. DEVEREUX G. (1972). Ethnopsychanalyse complémentariste. Paris, Flammarion.
  105. DORER M. (1932). Historische grundlagen der Psychoanalyse, Leipzig, Felix Meiner.
  106. DRIVER J. & VUILLEUMIER P. (2001). Perceptual awareness and its loss to unilateral neglect and extinction. Dans The Cognitive Neuroscience of Consciousness, dir. S. Dehaene et L. Naccache, Cambridge (MA), MIT Press.
  107. EDELMAN G.M. (1987). Neural darwinism: the theory of neuronal group selection, New York, Basic Books.
  108. EDELMAN G.M. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire. On the matter of mind, New York, Basic Books.EDELMAN G.M. (1992). Biologie de la conscience, Paris, Odile Jacob (traduction française de Bright air, brilliant fire).
  109. EDELMAN G.M. (2004). Wider than the sky: the phenomenal gift of consciousness, Yale Univ. Press.
  110. EDELMAN G.M. (2006). Second nature: brain science and human knowledge, Yale University Press.
  111. ERDELYI M. H. (1974). A new look at the New Look: Perceptual defense and vigilance. Psychological Review, 81,1-25.
  112. ERDELYI M.H. (1985). Psychoanalysis: Freud's cognitive psychology, New York, W.H. Freeman and Company.
  113. ERDELYI M.H. (1996). The Recovery of Unconscious Memories, Chicago & London, University of Chicago Press.
  114. ERDELYI M.H. (2004). Subliminal perception and its cognates: Theory, indeterminacy, and time, Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 73-91.
  115. ERIKSEN C. W. (1960). Discrimination and learning without awareness: a methodological survey and evaluation. Psychological Review, 67, 279–300.
  116. FADIGA L., FOGASSI L., PAVESI G. & RIZZOLATTI G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2608-2611.
  117. FARAH M.J. (1995). Current issues in the neuropsychology of image generation. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1455-1471.
  118. FAUST M. & CHIARELLO C. (1998). Sentence context and lexical ambiguity resolution by the two hemispheres. Neuropsychologia, 36, 827-836.
  119. FAUST M.E. & GERNSBACHER M.A. (1996). Cerebral mechanisms for suppression of inappropriate information during sentence comprehension. Brain and Language, 53, 234-259.
  120. FAZIO R.H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: an overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 115-141.
  121. FEDIDA P. (1977). Corps du vide et espace de séance, Paris, Jean-Pierre Delarge.
  122. FEDIDA P. (1990). A propos de l’article de D. Widlöcher. Revue internationale de psychopathologie, 2, 357-364.
  123. FEDIDA P. (2000). Le canular de la neuropsychanalyse. La Recherche, hors série 3.
  124. FEDIDA P. (2000). Par où commence le corps humain: retour sur la régression, Paris, PUF.
  125. FEDIDA P. & WIDLÖCHER D. (1990). Revue Internationale de Psychopathologie, 1, 4.
  126. FEDIDA P. & WIDLÖCHER D. (1995). Actualité des modèles freudiens: langage - image – pensée. Dans Monographies de la Revue Internationale de Psychopathologie, Paris, PUF, pp. 39–54.
  127. FISHER C. (1954). Dreams and perception. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 2, 389-445.
  128. FISHER C. (1957). A Study of the Preliminary Stages of the Construction of Dreams and Images. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 5, 5-60.
  129. FOTOPOULOU A. (2008). False selves in neuropsychological rehabilitation: the challenge of confabulation. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 18(5-6), 541-565.
  130. FOTOPOULOU A., CONWAY M.A., SOLMS M., TYRER S. & KOPELMAN M. (2008). Self-serving confabulation in prose recall. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1429-1441.
  131. FOTOPOULOU A., SOLMS M. & TURNBULL O.H. (2004). Wishful reality distortions in confabulation. Neuropsychologia, 42, 727-744.
  132. FOTOPOULOU A., TSAKIRIS M., HAGGARD P., VAGOPOULOU A., RUDD A. & KOPELMAN M. (2008). The role of motor intention in motor awareness: an experimental study on anosognosia for hemiplegia. Brain, 131(12), 3432-3442.
  133. FOX P.T., PARDO J.V., PETERSEN S.E. & RAICHLE M.E (1987). Supplementary motor and premotor responses to actual and imagined hand movements with positron emission tomography. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 13, 1433.
  134. FRACKOWIAK R.S.J. (1994). Functional mapping of verbal memory and language. Trends in Neuroscience, 17, 109-115.
  135. FREEDMAN D.J., RIESENHUBER M., POGGIO T. & MILLER E.K. (2003). A comparison of primate prefrontal and inferior temporal cortices during visual categorization. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5235-5246.
  136. FREUD A. (1936/1949). Le moi et les mécanismes de défense, trad. A. Berman, Paris, PUF.
  137. FREUD S. (1891/1983). Contribution à la conception des aphasies, trad. C. Van Reeth, Paris, PUF.
  138. FREUD S. (1891/2002). Over Afasie, trad. néerlandaise de Contribution à la conception des aphasies par G. Van de Vijver et F. Geerardyn, Gent, Idesça.
  139. FREUD S. (1893/1962). Charcot. Dans Standard Edition, 3, Londres, Hogarth.
  140. FREUD S. (1894/1973). Les psychonévroses de défense. Dans Névrose, psychose et perversion, trad. J. Laplanche, Paris, PUF.
  141. FREUD S. (1895/1956). Esquisse d’une psychologie scientifique. Dans La naissance de la psychanalyse, trad. A. Berman, Paris, PUF, 308-396.
  142. FREUD S. (1895/1966). Project for a scientific psychology. Dans Standard Edition, 1, Londres, Hogarth, 281–397.
  143. FREUD S. (1895/1992). Ontwerp van een natuurwetenschappelijke psychologie, trad. néerlandaise du Projet d'une psychologie scientifique de Freud, Gent, ldesça.
  144. FREUD S. (1896/1973). L'hérédité et l'étiologie des névroses. Dans Névrose, psychose et perversion, Paris, PUF.
  145. FREUD S. (1900/1969). L’interprétation des rêves, trad. I. Meyerson et D. Berger, Paris, PUF.
  146. FREUD S. (1901/1953). Psychopathologie de la vie quotidienne, trad. S. Jankélévitch, Paris, Payot.
  147. FREUD S. (1905a/1988). Le mot d’esprit et sa relation avec l’inconscient, Paris, Gallimard.
  148. FREUD S. (1905b/2006). Trois essais sur la théorie sexuelle, vi, PUF.
  149. FREUD S. (1911/1989). Formulations sur les deux principes de l’advenir psychique. Dans Œuvres complètes, volume XI, Paris, PUF.
  150. FREUD S. (1915/1969). Métapsychologie, trad. J. Laplanche et J.-B. Pontalis, Paris, Gallimard.
  151. FREUD S. (1915a/1969). L’inconscient. Dans Métapsychologie, trad. J. Laplanche et J.-B. Pontalis, Paris, Gallimard.
  152. FREUD S. (1915b/1969). Le refoulement. Dans Métapsychologie, trad. J. Laplanche et J.-B. Pontalis, Paris, Gallimard.
  153. FREUD S. (1915c/1994). Actuelles sur la guerre et la mort. Dans Œuvre Complète, T. XIII Paris, PUF.
  154. FREUD S. (1916-1917/1959). Introduction à la psychanalyse, trad. S. Jankélévitch, Paris, Payot.
  155. FREUD S. (1919/1954). Cinq psychanalyses, trad. M. Bonaparte et R. Loewenstein, Paris, PUF.
  156. FREUD S. (1920/1971). Au-delà du principe de plaisir. Dans Essais de psychanalyse, trad. J. Laplanche et J. B. Pontalis, Paris, Payot.
  157. FREUD S. (1923/1991). «  Psychanalyse » et « théorie de la libido ». Dans Œuvres Complètes, T. XVI, Paris, PUF.
  158. FREUD S. (1925/1984). S. Freud présenté par lui-même, Paris, Gallimard.
  159. FREUD S. (1938/1950). Abrégé de psychanalyse, trad. A. Berman, Paris, PUF.
  160. FREUD S. (1956). Naissance de la psychanalyse, trad. A. Berman, Paris, PUF.
  161. FREUD S. (1993). Cinq conférences. Dans Œuvres complètes, X, Paris, PUF.
  162. FREUD S. & BREUER J. (1895/1973). Études sur l’hystérie, trad. A. Berman, Paris, PUF.
  163. FRIEDERICI A.D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 78-84.
  164. FRIEDMAN L., KENNY J.T., WISE A.L., WU D., STUVE T.A., MILLER D.A., JESBERGER J.A. & LEWIN J.B. (1998). Brain activation during silent word generation evaluated with functional MRI. Brain and Language, 64, 231-256.
  165. FRIEDMAN-HILL S.R., ROBERTSON L.C., DESIMONE R. & UNGERLEIDER L.G. (2003). Posterior parietal cortex and the filtering of distracters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 4263-4268.
  166. FRITH C.D. (1992). The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia, Hove, Lawrence Erlbaum.
  167. FRITH C.D., BLAKEMORE S.J., & WOLPERT D.M. (2000). Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 357–363.
  168. FROST R. (1998). Towards a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71–99.
  169. GAINOTTI G. (2000). What the locus of brain lesion tells us about the nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders. A review. Cortex, 36, 539-559.
  170. GALDERISI S., HERRMANN W.M., KINOSHITA T., KOUKKOU M., MUCCI A., PASCUAL-MARQUI R.D., SAITO N., WACKERMANN J., WINTERER G. & KOENIG T. (2005). EEG microstate duration and syntax in acute, medication-naive, first-episode schizophrenia: a multi-center study. Psychiatry re search, 138(2), 141-156.
  171. GALLESE V. (2000). The inner sense of action: agency and motor representations. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 23-40.
  172. GALLESE V. & GOLDMAN A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 493-501.
  173. GASKELL M.G. & MARSLEN-WILSON W.D. (1997). Integrating form and meaning: a distributed model of speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 613-656.
  174. GAZZANIGA M.S. (1985). The Social Brain – Discovering the Networks of the Mind, New York, Basic Books.
  175. GAZZANIGA M.S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication. Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition?. Brain, 123, 1293-1326.
  176. GEERARDYN F., VAN DE VIJVER G. & BAZAN A. (2002). Éditorial de la traduction néerlandaise du livre de S. Freud sur les aphasies, Freud’s afasie-studie: historische en epistemologische kanttekeningen (« L’étude de Freud sur l’aphasie: remarques historiques et épistémologiques »), Gent, ldesça, xi-xxix.
  177. GEORGIEFF N. (1996). Organisation et représentation de l'action dans la schizophrénie. Dans L'Encéphale, XXII, III, 108-115.
  178. GEORGIEFF N. (2000). Neuropsychopathologie cognitive sociale de l'action: apport à l'étude des symptômes positifs de la schizophrénie. Intellectica, 2, 31, 191-225.
  179. GEORGIEFF N. (2004). Qu’est-ce que la schizophrénie?,Paris, Dunod, Coll. Les topos, 128 p.
  180. GEORGIEFF N. (2005). Pour un échange entre psychanalyse et sciences de l'esprit. L'Évolution Psychiatrique, 70, 1, 63-85.
  181. GEORGIEFF N. (2006). Freud est-il soluble dans les neurosciences ? Dans L’inconscient, Le journal du CNRS, mars, 194 - http://www2.cnrs.fr/presse/journal/2720.htm
  182. GEORGIEFF N. (2007). Psychanalyse et neurosciences du lien: nouvelles conditions pour une rencontre entre psychanalyse et neurosciences. Revue française de psychanalyse, 71/2, 501-516.
  183. GEORGIEFF N. (2008). Qu’est-ce que l’autisme?, Paris, Dunod, Coll. Les topos, 128 p.
  184. GEORGIEFF N. & JEANNEROD M. (1998). Beyond consciousness of external reality. a “who” system for consciousness of action and self-consciousness. Conscious Cognition, 7, 465-477.
  185. GERNSBACHER M.A. & FAUST M.E. (1991). The mechanism of suppression: a component of general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 2, 245-262.
  186. GERNSBACHER M.A. & ROBERTSON R.R.W. (1995). Reading skill and suppression revisited. Psychological Science, 6, 165-169.
  187. GERNSBACHER M.A., VARNER K.R. & FAUST M.E. (1990). Investigating differences in general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 3, 430-445.
  188. GIAMPIERI-DEUTSCH P., éd. (2007). Geist, Gehirn, Verhalten: Sigmund Freud und die modernen Wissenschaften, Würzburg, Königshausen & Neumann.
  189. GIEDD J.N., BLUMENTHAL J., JEFFRIES N.O., CASTELLANOS F.X., LIU H., ZIJDENBOS A., PAUS T., EVANS A.C. & RAPOPORT J.L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 10, 861-863.
  190. GILL M.M. (1963). Topography and systems in psychoanalytic theory, (Psychological Issues, 10), New York, International Universities Press.
  191. GILL M.M. (1979). The analysis of the transference, Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 27 (Suppl.), 263-288.
  192. GILL M.M. (1994). Psychoanalysis in transition: a personal view, Hillsdale, NJ, Analytic Press.
  193. GLOVER S. (2004). Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, p.4.
  194. GOLSE B. & MISSONNIER S. (2005), Récit, attachement et psychanalyse. Pour une clinique de la narrativité, Toulouse, Érès.
  195. GORFEIN D.S., BERGER S. & BUBKA A. (2000). The selection of homograph meaning: word association when context changes. Memory and Cognition, 28, 766-73.
  196. GREEN A. (1992).Un psychanalyste face aux neurosciences. La Recherche, 247, 23, 1166-1174.
  197. GREEN A. (1995). La causalité psychique - Entre nature et culture, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  198. GREEN A. (2002). Idées directrices pour une psychanalyse contemporaine. Paris, PUF.
  199. GREEN M.F. & PRESTON M. (1981). Reinforcement of vocal correlates of auditory feedback: a case study. British Journal Of Psychiatry, 139, 204-208
  200. GREENWALD A.G.,DRAINE S.C. & ABRAMSR.L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273, 1699-702.
  201. GRUSH R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 377-442.
  202. HAGGARD P. (2005). Conscious intention and motor cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 6, 290-295.
  203. HAGGARD P. & WHITFORD B. (2004). Supplementary motor area provides an efferent signal for sensory suppression. Cognitive Brain Research, 19, 52-58.
  204. HAGOORT P. (1993). Impairments of lexical-semantic processing in aphasia: evidence from the processing of lexical ambiguities. Brain and Language, 45, 189-232.
  205. HALGREN E. (1992). Emotional neurophysiology of the amygdala within the context of human cognition. Dans The amygdala, dir. J.P. Aggleton, Chichester, Wiley, 191-228.
  206. HAMKER F.H. (2003). The reentry hypothesis: linking eye movements to visual perception. Journal of Vision, 3, 808-816.
  207. HARTMANN E. (1973). The functions of sleep, New Haven and London, Yale University Press.
  208. HARTMANN E. (1995). Making connections in a safe place: is dreaming psychotherapy? Dreaming, 5, 213–228.
  209. HARTMANN E. (2000). The waking-to-dreaming continuum and the effects of emotion, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 947–950.
  210. HASKELL R.E. (1999). Between the lines: unconscious meaning in everyday conversation, New York, Insight Books, Plenum Press.
  211. HASKELL R.E. (2001). Deep listening, hidden meanings in everyday conversation, Cambridge (Mass.), Perseus Publishing.
  212. HASKELL R.E. (2003). Sub-literal (SubLit) language and cognition. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 24, 347-400.
  213. HEINKS-MALDONADO T. H., MATHALON D.H., GRAY M. & FORD J.M. (2005). Fine-tuning of auditory cortex during speech production. Psychophysiology, 42, 2, 180-190.
  214. HEIT G., SMITH M. E. & HALGREN E. (1988). Neural encoding of individual words and faces by the human hippocampus and amygdala. Nature, 333, 773-775.
  215. HICKOK G. & POEPPEL D. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 131-138.
  216. HILLIS A.E. & CARAMAZZA A. (1995). Representation of grammatical categories of words in the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 396-407.
  217. HOBSON J. (1988). The dreaming brain, New York, Basic Books.
  218. HOBSON J.A. & MCCARLEY R.W. (1977). The brain as a dream state generator: an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1335-1348.
  219. HOCHMANN J. & JEANNEROD M. (1991). Esprit où es-tu ? Psychanalyse et neuroscience, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  220. HOLENDER D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision and visual masking: a survey and appraisal. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1-66.
  221. HOROWITZ M.J. (1988). Psychodynamics and cognition, Chicago, The University of Chicago Press
  222. HOUDE J.F., NAGARAJAN S.S., SEKIHARA K. & MERZENICH M.M. (2002). Modulation of the auditory cortex during speech: an meg study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1125-1138.
  223. HUGDAHL K., RUND B.R., LUND A., ASBJORNSEN A., EGELAND J., ERSLAND L., LANDRO N.I., RONESS A., STORDAL K.I., SUNDET K. & THOMSEN T. (2004). Brain activation measured with fmri during a mental arithmetic task in schizophrenia and major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 286-293.
  224. HUGLO M.-P. (décembre 2001). Du palimpseste à l’écho. Un homme qui dort de Georges Perec. Le Cabinet d’amateur, <http://www.cabinetperec.org/articles/ huglo/>.
  225. HUSSERL E. (1935-36/1962). La crise des sciences européennes et la phénoménologie transcendantale, trad. G. Granel, Paris, Gallimard.
  226. ISENBERG N., SILBERSWEIG D., ENGELIEN A., EMMERICH S., MALAVADE K., BEATTIE B., LEON A. C. & STERN E. (1999). Linguistic threat activates the human amygdale. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 10456-10459.
  227. ISHAI A., UNGERLEIDER L.G., MARTIN A., SCHOUTEN J.L. & HAXBY J.V. (1999). Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 9379-9384.
  228. IVERSON J.M. & GOLDIN-MEADOW S. (1998). Why People Gesture When They Speak. Nature, 396, 228.
  229. IVRY R. B. & JUSTUS T.C. (2001). A neural instantiation of the motor theory of speech perception. Trends in Neurosciences, 24, 513-515.
  230. JAMES T.W., CULHAM J.C., HUMPHREY G.K., MILNER A.D. & GOODALE, M.A. (2003). Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object directed grasping: an fMRI study. Brain, 126, 11, 2475-2763.
  231. JEANNEROD M. (1990). Traitement conscient et non conscient de l’information perceptive. Revue Internationale de Psychopathologie, 1, 13-34.JEANNEROD M. (2002). Le cerveau intime, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  232. JEANNEROD M. (1994). The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187-245.
  233. JEANNEROD M. (1994). The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187-245.
  234. JEANNEROD M. (1999). A dichotomous visual brain?. Psyche, 5. <http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v5/psyche-5-25-jeannerod.html>.
  235. JEANNEROD M. (2002). L’inconscient à l’épreuve des neurosciences.Le Monde Diplomatique, septembre.
  236. JEANNEROD M. (2002). Le cerveau intime, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  237. JEANNEROD M. (2003). The mechanism of self-recognition in humans. Behavioural Brain Research, 142, 1–15.
  238. JEANNEROD M. (2007). L’homme sans visage et autres récits de neurologie quotidienne, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  239. JEANNEROD M. & DECETY J. (1995). Mental motor imagery: a window into the representational stages of action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 727-732.
  240. JEANNEROD M. & GEORGIEFF N. (2000). Psychanalyse et science(s). Institut des Sciences Cognitives Working Papers, 4; http://www.isc.cnrs.fr/wp/wp00-4.htm.
  241. JEANNEROD M. & JACOB P. (2005). Visual cognition: a new look at the two-visual systems model. Neuropsychologia, 43, 301-312.
  242. JEANNEROD M. & PACHERIE E. (2004). Agency, simulation and self-identification. Mind and Language, 19, 2, 2004, 113-146.
  243. JONES E. (1958). La vie et l’œuvre de Sigmund Freud, PUF, Paris.
  244. JONES, E. (1953). Sigmund Freud Life and Work, Volume one: The young Freud, 1856-1900. London, The Hogarth Press.
  245. KANDEL E. (1999). Biology and the future of psychoanalysis: a new intellectual framework for psychiatry revisited. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 4, 505-524.
  246. KANDEL E. (2007). À la recherche de la mémoire, une nouvelle théorie de l’esprit, trad. M. Filoche, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  247. KANDEL E., SCHWARTZ J.H. & JESSELL T.M. (2000). Principles of neural science, New York, McGraw-Hill.
  248. KANT I. (1787/1944). Critique de la raison pure, trad. A.Tremesaygues et B. Pacaud, Paris, PUF.
  249. KAPLAN-SOLMS K. et SOLMS M. (2000). Clinical studies in neuro-psychoanalysis. Introduction to a depth neuropsychology, Madison (CT), International Universities Press.
  250. KAUFFMAN S.A. (1993). The origins of order: self-organization and selection in evolution, New York, Oxford University Press.
  251. KELLER P.-H. (2003). À propos de quelques analogies du texte freudien. Revue française de psychanalyse, 1, 67, 287-297.
  252. KLEIN VILLA K., SHEVRIN H., SNODGRASS M., BAZAN A. & BRAKEL L.A.W. (2007). Testing Freud’s hypothesis that word forms and word meanings are functionally distinct in the unconscious: Subliminal primary process cognition and its links to personality. Neuro-Psychoanalysis, 2, 117-138.
  253. KLIMESCH W., SAUSENG P. & HANSLMAYR S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Research Reviews, 53, 63-88.
  254. KNOCKAERT V. (2007). Over de objectiveerbaarheid van het psychisme als levende structuur. Een epistemologische studie van Feuds publieke en private geschriften van 1890 tot en met 1900. Thèse de doctorat en philosophie non publiée, Université de Gand, Belgique.
  255. KOLEN F. & VAN DE VIJVER G. (2007). Philosophy of biology: naturalistic or transcendental?. Acta Biotheoretica, 55, 35-46.
  256. KOUKKOU M. & LEUZINGER-BOHLEBER M. (1992). Psychoanalysis and neurophysiology: a look at case material from the two theoretical perspectives. An interdisciplinary understanding of some basic psychoanalytic concepts. Dans Two Butterflies on My Head ... Psychoanalysis in the interdisciplinary dialogue, éds. M. Leuzinger-Bohleber, H. Schneider & R. Pfeifer, Berlin, Springer Verlag, 133-179.
  257. KOUKKOU M., LEHMANN D., WACKERMANN J., DVORAK I. & HENGGELER B. (1993). Dimensional complexity of EEG brain mechanisms in untreated schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 33, 397-407.
  258. KOUKKOU M., LEUZINGER-BOHLEBER M. & MERTENS W. (1998). Erinnerung von Wirklichkeiten. Psychoanalyse und Neurowissenschaften im Dialog. (Vol. 1: Bestandsaufnahme; Vol 2: Folgerungen ftir die psychoanalytische Praxis), Stuttgart, Verlag Internationale Psychoanalyse.
  259. KUBIE L.S. (1930). A theoretical application to some neurological problems of the properties of excitation waves which move in closed circuits. Brain, 53, 166-177.
  260. KUBIE L.S. (1936). Relation of the conditioned reflex to psychoanalytic technique. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 32, 1137-1142.
  261. KUBIE L.S. (1941). Physiological approaches to the concept of anxiety. Psychosomatic Medicine, 3, 263-276.
  262. KUBIE L.S. (1941). Repetitive Core of Neuroses, Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 10, 23-43.
  263. KUBIE L.S. (1951). The relationship of symbolic function in language formation and in neuroses. Dans Cybernetics - circular, causal and feedback mechanisms in biological and social systems, ed. H. von Foerster,Transactions of the seventh conference, New York: Josiah Macy Jr. Foundation.
  264. LACAN J. (1938/2001). Les complexes familiaux dans la formation de l'individu. Dans Autres Ecrits, Paris, Seuil, 23-84.
  265. LACAN J. (1953/2001).Discours et réponse aux interventions, Congrès de Rome. La Psychanalyse. Dans Autres Écrits, Paris, Seuil.
  266. LACAN J. (1953-1954/1975). Le séminaire, livre I, Les écrits techniques de Freud, Paris, Seuil
  267. LACAN J. (1954-1955/1978). Le s éminaire , livreii, Le moi dans la théorie de Freud et dans la technique de la psychanalyse, Paris, Seuil.
  268. LACAN J. (1955-1956/1981). Le séminaire, livre III, Les psychoses, Paris, Seuil.
  269. LACAN J. (1957/1999). L’instance de la lettre dans l’inconscient ou la raison depuis Freud. Dans Écrits I, Paris, Seuil.
  270. LACAN J. (1960-1961/1991). Le séminaire, livre VIII, Le transfert, Paris, Seuil.
  271. LACAN J. (1962-1963/2004). Le séminaire, livre X, L’angoisse, Paris, Seuil.
  272. LACAN J. (1964/1973). Le séminaire, livre XI, Les quatre concepts fondamentaux de la psychanalyse, Paris, Seuil.
  273. LACAN J. (1964/1999). Position de l'inconscient, Congrès de Bonneval. Dans Écrits II, Paris, Seuil, pp. 309-330.
  274. LACAN J. (1966). La science et la vérité. Dans Écrits, Paris, Seuil.
  275. LACAN J. (1966/1999). D'un syllabaire après coup. Dans Écrits II, Paris, Seuil, pp. 196-202.
  276. LACAN J. (1972-1973/1975). Le séminaire, livre XX, Encore, Paris, Seuil.
  277. LAKOFF G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: what categories reveal about the mind, Chicago, Chicago University Press.
  278. LANDAU B. & JACKENDOFF R. (1993). “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 217-265.
  279. LEBRUN J.-P. (1993). De la maladie médicale, Bruxelles, De Boeck-Wesemael.
  280. LECHEVALIER B. & LECHEVALIER B. (1998), Le corps et le sens, Paris, Delachaux & Niestlé, p. 292.
  281. LEDOUX J.E. (1993). Emotional memory systems in the brain. Behavioural Brain Research, 58, 69-79.
  282. LEDOUX J.E. (1994). Emotion, memory and the brain. Scientific American, 6, 3, 2-39.
  283. LEDOUX J.E. (1996). The emotional brain, New York, Simon & Schuster.
  284. LENAY C. (2006). Enaction, externalisme et suppléance perceptive. Intellectica, 43, 27–52.
  285. LEUZINGER-BOHLEBER M. & PFEIFER R. (2007). Recollecting the past in the present: memory in the dialogue between psychoanalyis and cognitive science, dans Psychoanalysis and Neurocience Mauro Mancia, 63-95.
  286. LEUZINGER-BOHLEBER M., DREHER A.U. & CANESTRI J., éds. (2003). Pluralism and Unity? Methods of Research in Psychoanalysis. London, International Psychoanalytical Association, 149-167.
  287. LEVELT W.J.M. (2001). Spoken word production: a theory of lexical access. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 13464-13471.
  288. LEVELT W.J.M., ROELOFS A. & MEYER A.S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 22, 1-38.
  289. Levin F.M. (1991). Mapping the mind. The intersection of psychoanalysis and neuroscience, Hillsdale NJ/London, The Analytic Press, 264 p.
  290. Levin F.M. (2004). Psyche and brain: the biology of talking cures. Madison CT, International Universities. Press, 314 p.
  291. Levin F.M. (2009).Emotion and the psychodynamics of the cerebellum : a neuro-psychoanalytic analysis and synthesis, London, Karnac.
  292. LIBERMAN A.M., COOPER F.S. & SHANKWEILER D.P. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431-461.
  293. LIBERMAN A. M. & MATTINGLY I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1-36.
  294. LIDDLE P.F., FRISTON K.J., FRITH C.D., JONES T., HIRSCH S.R. & FRACKOWIAK R.S.J. (1992).Patterns of regional cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 179-186.
  295. LOTSTRA F. (2007). Psychanalyse et neuroscience: la fin d’un schisme? Revue médicale de Bruxelles, 28, 2, 91-96.
  296. LUBORSKY L., MINTZ J., AUERBACH A., CHRISTOPH P., BACHRACH H., TODD T., JOHNSON M., COHEN M., O'BRIEN C.P. (1980) Predicting the outcome of psychotherapy. Findings of the Penn psychotherapy project. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 471-481.
  297. LUCE P.A. & PISONI D.B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: the neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 1-36.
  298. LUPPINO G., MATELLI M., CAMARDA R. & RIZZOLATTI G. (1993). Corticocortical connections of area F3 (SMAproper) and area F6 (pre-SMA) in the macaque monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 338, 114-140.
  299. LURIA A.R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man, London, Tavistock Publications.
  300. LURIA A.R. (1973). The working brain: an introduction to neuropsychology. Harmondsworth, Penguin.
  301. MACNEILAGE P.F. (1998). The frame/content theory of evolution of speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 499-511.
  302. MAGISTRETTI P. & ANSERMET F. (2007). La plasticité neuronale: un nouveau paradigme entre neurosciences et psychanalyse. Psychiatrie, Sciences Humaines, Neurosciences, 5, 3, 138-143.
  303. MAQUET P., PETERS J.M., AERTS J., DELFIORE G., DEGUELDRE C., LUXEN A. & FRANCK G. (1996). Functional neuroanatomy of human rapideye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature, 383, 163-166.
  304. MARTIN A., WIGGS C.L., UNGERLEITER L. G. & HAXBY J. V. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649-652.
  305. MATHALON D.H., FAUSTMAN W.O. & FORD J.M. (2002). N400 and automatic semantic processing abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 641-648.
  306. MCCLELLAND J.L. & ELMAN J.L. (1986). The trace model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1-86.
  307. MCGONIGLE D.J., HÄNNINEN R., SALENIUS S., HARI R., FRACKOWIAK R.S.J. & FRITH C.D. (2002). Whose arm is it anyway? An fmri case study of supernumerary phantom limb. Brain, 125, 1265-1274.
  308. MCGUIRE P.K., SHAH G.M.S., & MURRAY R.M. (1993). Increased blood flow in broca’s area during auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Lancet, 342, 703-706.
  309. MCGUIRE P.K., SILBERSWEIG D.A., MURRAY R.M., DAVID A.S., FRACKOWIAK R.S.J. & FRITH C.D. (1996). Functional anatomy of inner speech and auditory verbal imagery. Psychological Medicine, 26, 29-38.
  310. MEDIN D. L., GOLDSTONE R. L. & GENTNER D. (1990). Similarity involving attributes and relations: judgments of similarity and difference are not inverses. Psychological Science, vol. 1 (1), 64-69.
  311. MELMAN C. (2001). Quartier Lacan, témoignages sur Jacques Lacan. Paris, Denoël , collectif « L’espace analytique».
  312. MENECHAL J. & GEORGIEFF N. (2002). Psychanalyse et sciences cognitives, Paris, Dunod.
  313. MERIKLE P.M. (1982). Unconscious perception revisited. Perception & Psychophysics, 31, 298-301.
  314. MERLEAU-PONTY M. (1942). La structure du comportement. Paris, PUF.
  315. MERLEAU-PONTY M. (1945). Phénoménologie de la perception, Paris, Gallimard.
  316. MILLER J.A. (2000). Biologie lacanienne et événement de corps. La cause freudienne, 44, 7-59.
  317. MILLER G., GALANTER E. & PRIBRAM K. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior, New York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
  318. MILNER D.A. & GOODALE M. (1995). The visual brain in action, Oxford, Oxford University Press.
  319. MIOZZO M. & CARAMAZZA A. (1997). On knowing the auxiliary of a verb that cannot Be named: evidence for the independence of grammatical and phonological aspects of lexical knowledge. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 160-166.
  320. MISSONNIER S. (2003). La consultation thérapeutique périnatale, Toulouse, Érès.
  321. MISSONNIER S., GOLSE B. & SOULÉ M. (2004). La grossesse, l'enfant virtuel et la parentalité. Éléments de psycho(patho)logie périnatale, Paris, PUF.
  322. MODELL A. (1993). The Private Self, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.
  323. MORITZ S., MERSMANN K., KLOSS M., JACOBSEN D., ANDRESEN B., KRAUSZ M., PAWLIK K. & NABE D. (2001). Enhanced semantic priming in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients using a word pronunciation task. Schizophrenia Research, 8, 301-305.
  324. MURRAY E.A. (1996). What have ablation studies told us about the neural substrates of stimulus memory?. Seminars in Neurosciences, 8, 13-22.
  325. NACCACHE L. (2006). Le Nouvel inconscient: Freud, le Christophe Colomb des neurosciences, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  326. NORRIS D. (1994). Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52, 189-234.
  327. OJEMANN G. A. (1979). Individual variability in cortical localization of language. Journal of Neurosurgery, 50, 164-169.
  328. OJEMANN G. A. (1983). Brain organization of language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 2, 189-230.
  329. OJEMANN G. A. (1991). Cortical organization of language. Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 2281-2287.
  330. Olds D.D. (2003). Affect as a sign system. Neuro-psychoanalysis, 5, 195-199.
  331. ONIFER W. & SWINNEY D.A. (1981). Accessing lexical ambiguities during sentence comprehension: effects of frequency of meaning and contextual bias. Memory & Cognition, 9, 225-236.
  332. OSGOOD C.E. (1980). Lectures on language performance, New York, Springer.
  333. OSTOW M. (1954). A psychoanalytic contribution to the study of brain function – I. the frontal lobes. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 23, 317-338.
  334. OSTOW M. (1955a). A psychoanalytic contribution to the study of brain function – II. the temporal lobes – III, Synthesis. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 24, 383-423.
  335. OSTOW M. (1955b). Behavior correlates of neural function, American Scientist, 43, 127-133.
  336. OSTOW M. (1955c). Psychic contents and processes of the brain, Psychosomatic Medicine, 17, 396-406.
  337. OSTOW M. (1956). Psychoanalysis and the brain. Acta Medica Orientalia, 15, 167-176.
  338. OSTOW M. (1962). Drugs in psychoanalysis and psychotherapy, New York, Basic Books.
  339. OSTOW M. (1980). The psychodynamic approach to drug therapy, New York, Van Nostrand Reinhold.
  340. OUSS-RYNGAERT L. (2004). L’intersubjectivité comme paradigme de l’intérêt des liens neurosciences-psychanalyse. Psychiatrie Française, 1, 37-61.
  341. OUSS - RYNGAERT L. (2007). Impact des neurosciences sur la pratique psychanalytique: la double lecture comme clinique " neuro-psychanalytique ". Revue Française de Psychanalyse, 71, 419-436.
  342. OUSS-RYNGAERT L. & ROSSETTI Y. (2006). Psychologie, psychopathologie et neurosciences: un bébé en développement?, dans Les émotions (autour) du bébé, ed. L. Dugnat, Ramonville-Saint-Agne, Érès, 63-77.
  343. OWEN A.M., MILNER B., PETRIDES M. & EVANS A.C. (1996). Memory for object features versus memory for object location: a positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93, 9212-9217.
  344. PALOMBO S.R. (1978). Dreaming and memory: a new information-processing model, New York, Basic Books.
  345. PALOMBO S.R. (1984). Recovery of early memories associated with reported dream imagery. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 12, 1508-11.
  346. PALOMBO S.R. (1992). Connectivity and condensation in dreaming. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 40, 1139-1159.
  347. PALOMBO S.R. (1999). The emergent ego: complexity and coevolution in the psychoanalytic process, Madison, Conn., International Universities Press.
  348. PANKSEPP J. (1985). Mood changes. Dans Handbook of clinical neurology, 45, ed. P.Vinken, C. Bruyn et H. Klawans, Amsterdam, Elsevier, pp. 271-285.
  349. Panksepp J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York, Oxford University Press, 466 p.
  350. PANKSEPP J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions, Oxford, Oxford University Press.
  351. Panksepp J. (1999). Emotions as viewed by psychoanalysis and neuroscience: an exercise in consilience. Neuro-Psychoanalysis, 1, 15-38.
  352. PAUL S.T., KELLAS G., MARTIN M. & CLARK M.B. (1992). Influence of contextual features on the activation of ambiguous word meanings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 703-717.
  353. PAULHUS D.L. (1991). Measurement and control of response bias. Dans Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes, ed. J.P. Robinson, P.R. Shaver et L.S. Wrightsman, New York, Academic Press, 17-59.
  354. PEIRCE. C.S. (1931/1966). Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce, 8 vols., dir. C. Hartshorne, P. Weiss et A.W. Burks, Cambridge (Mass.), Harvard University Press.
  355. PETERFREUND E. (1971). Information systems and psychoanalysis: an evolutionary biological approach to psychoanalytic theory. Psychological Issues, 7 (1 & 2), Monograph 25/26.
  356. PETERFREUND E. (1975a). The need for a new general theoretical frame of reference for psychoanalysis, Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 44, 534-549.
  357. PETERFREUND E. (1975b). How does the analyst listen? On models and strategies in the psychoanalytic process. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Science, 4, 59-101.
  358. PETERFREUND E. (1983). The process of psychoanalytic therapy. Hillsdale, NJ, The Analytic Press.
  359. PISELLA L., GREA H., TILIKETE C., VIGHETTO A., DESMURGET M., RODE G., BOISSON D. & ROSSETTI Y. (2000). An automatic pilot for the hand in the human posterior parietal cortex towards a reinterpretation of optic ataxia. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 7, 729-736.
  360. POLDRACK R.A., WAGNER A.D., PRULL M.W., DESMOND J.E., GLOVER G.H. & GABRIELI J.D. (1999). Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage, 10, 15-35.
  361. POMMIER G. (2004). Comment les neurosciences démontrent la psychanalyse. Paris, Flammarion.
  362. Pompeiano O. (1979). Cholinergic activation of reticular and vestibular mechanisms controlling posture and eye movements. Dans The Reticular Formation Revisited, ed. J.A. Hobson et N. Brazier, New York, Raven.
  363. PÖTZL O. (1917). Experimentell erregte Traumbilder in ihren beziehungen zum indirekten sehen. Zeitschrift fürdie gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 37, 3-4.
  364. PÖTZL O. (1960). The relationship between experimentally induced dream images and indirect vision, Monograph no. 7. Psychological Issues, 2, 41–120.
  365. PÖTZL O., ALLERS R. & TELER J. (1960). Preconscious stimulation in dreams, associations, and images: classical studies (Introduction by Charles Fisher), New York, International Universities Press
  366. PRIBRAM K. (1965). Freud’s project: An open biologically based model for psychoanalysis. Dans Psychoanalysis and current biological thought, ed. N.S. Greenfield et W.C. Lewis, The University of Wisconsin Press, 81–92.
  367. PRIBRAM K.H. (1969). Brain and behaviour, Hammondsworth, Penguin Books.
  368. PRIBRAM K.H. (1993). Rethinking neural networks: quantum fields and biological data, Hillsdale, N.J., Erlbaum.
  369. PRIBRAM K.H. & GILL M.M. (1976). Freud's "Project" re-assessed: preface to contemporary cognitive theory and neuropsychology, New York, Basic Books.
  370. PRIBRAM K.H.. & LURIA A.R. (1973). Psychophysiology of the frontal lobes, New York, Academic Press.
  371. PRICE C. J., WISE R.J.S., WARBURTON E.A., MOORE C.J., HOWARD D., PATTERSON K., FRACKOWIAK R.S.J. & FRISTON K. J. (1996). Hearing and saying. The functional neuro-anatomy of auditory word processing. Brain, 119, 919-931.
  372. PURCELL D.G., STEWART A. L. & STANOVICH K.E. (1983). Another look at semantic priming without awareness. Perception and Psychophysics, 34, 65-71.
  373. RAMACHANDRAN V.S. (1994). Phantom limbs, neglect syndromes, repressed memories, and freudian psychology. International Review of Neurobiology, 37, 291-333.
  374. RAPAPORT D. (1951). Towards a theory of thinking. Dans Organization and pathology of thought, dir. D. Rapaport, New York, Columbia University Press.
  375. REISER M.F. (1984). Mind, brain, body —Toward a convergence of psychoanalysis and neurobiology, New York, Basic Books.
  376. REISER M.F. (1985). Converging sectors of psychoanalysis and neurobiology: mutual challenge and opportunities. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 33, 11-34.
  377. REISER M.F. (1990). Memory in mind and brain. What dream imagery reveals, New York, Basic Books.
  378. RICCI P.T., ZELKOWICZ B.J., NEBES R.D., MELTZER C.C., MINTUN M.A. & BECKER J.T. (1999). Functional neuroanatomy of semantic memory: recognition of semantic associations. NeuroImage, 9, 88-96.
  379. RIZZOLATTI G. & ARBIB M.A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience, 21, 188-194.
  380. RIZZOLATTI G., FADIGA L., GALLESE V. & FOGASSI L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor action. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131-141.
  381. ROLAND P.E. (1984). Organization of motor control by the normal human brain. Human Neurobiology, 2, 205-216.
  382. ROLAND P.E., LARSEN B., LASSEN N.A. & SKINHOJ E. (1980). Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas in organization of voluntary movements in man. Journal of Neurophysiology, 43, 118-136.
  383. ROLLS E. T. (2000). Neurophysiology and functions of the primate amygdala, and the neural basis of emotion. Dans The amygdala: A functional analysis, dir. J.P. Aggleton, Oxford, Oxford University Press, 447-478.
  384. ROSSETTI Y. (1994). Inconscient: cérébral ou psychanalytique? Actualités Médicales Internationales - Psychiatrie, 168, 2493-2494.
  385. ROSSETTI Y. (1999). In search of immaculate perception: evidence from motor representation of space. Dans Towards a science of consciousness, dir.S. Hameroff, A. Kaszniak et D. Chalmers, Cambridge, mit Press, 141-148.
  386. ROSSETTI Y. & PISELLA L. (2000). L‘ego Légo: déconstruire ou reconstruire le cerveau-esprit? Intellectica, 31, 137-173.
  387. ROSSETTI Y. & PISELLA L. (2003). Mediate responses as direct evidence for intention: neuropsychology of not to-, not now- and not there-tasks. Dans Cognitive Neuroscience Perspectives on the Problem of Intentional Action, dir. S. Johnson, Cambridge, mit Press, 67-105.
  388. ROUSSELET G.-A., THORPE S.J. & FABRE-THORPE M. (2004). How parallel is visual processing in the ventral pathway? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 8, 363-370.
  389. ROUSSILLON R. (1992). Du baquet de Mesmer au « baquet » de S. Freud. Une archéologie du cadre et de la pratique psychanalytiques, Paris, PUF.
  390. ROUSSILLON R. (1999). Agonie, clivage et symbolisation, Paris, PUF, Collection « Le Fait psychanalytique ».
  391. ROUSSILLON R. (2003). Historicité et mémoire subjective. La troisième trace en mémoire entre psychanalyse et neurosciences, La Recherche, 67.
  392. ROUSSILLON R. (2005). Paradoxes et situations limites de la psychanalyse, Paris, PUF/Quadrige.
  393. ROUSSILLON R. (2007a). La représentance et l’actualisation pulsionnelle. Revue Française de Psychanalyse, 71, 339-357.
  394. ROUSSILLON R. (2007b). Logiques et archéologiques du cadre psychanalytique, Paris, PUF.
  395. ROUSSILLON R. (2008). Le transitionnel, le sexuel et la réflexivité. Paris, Dunod.
  396. ROUSSILLON R., CHABERT C., CICCONE A., FERRANT A., GEORGIEFF N. et ROMAN P. (2007). Manuel de psychologie et de psychopathologie clinique générale, Issy-les-Moulineaux, Elsevier-Masson.
  397. RUBINSTEIN B. (1965). Psychoanalytic Theory and the Mind-Body Problem. Dans Psychoanalysis and current biological thought, eds. N.S. Greenfield et W.C. Lewis, Madison, University of Wisconsin Press, 35-56.
  398. RUBINSTEIN B. (1980). The problem of confirmation in clinical psychoanalysis. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 28, 397–417.
  399. RUBINSTEIN B. (1999). Psychoanalysis and the Philosophy of Science, Collected Papers of Benjamin B. Rubinstein, International Universities Press.
  400. RYDING E., BRADVIK B. & INGVAR D.H. (1996). Silent speech activates prefrontal cortical regions asymmetrically, as well as speech-related areas in the dominant hemisphere. Brain and Language, 52, 435-451.
  401. SAVAGE-RUMBAUGH E.S. (1986). Ape language: from conditioned response to symbol, New York, Columbia University Press.
  402. SCHORE, A. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: the neurobiology of emotional development. Hillsdale NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  403. SCHORE, A. (2003). Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self, New York, W. W. Norton & Company, 300 p.
  404. SEGLAS J. (1892). Les troubles du langage chez les aliénés. Rueff, Paris.
  405. SEIDENBERG M.S., TANENHAUS M.K., LEIMAN J.M. & BIENKOWSKI M. (1982). Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: some limitations of knowledge-based processing. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 489-537.
  406. SEVERANCE E. & WASHBURN M.F. (1907). The loss of associative power in words after long fixation. American Journal of Psychology, 18, 182-186.
  407. SHEVRIN H. (1973). Brain wave correlates of subliminal stimulation, unconscious attention, primary-and secondary-process thinking and repressiveness. Psychological Issues, Monograph, 30, 8, 2, 56-87.
  408. SHEVRIN H. (1998). Why do we need to be conscious? A psychoanalytic answer. Dans Advanced Personality, dir. D.F. Barone, M. Hersen et V.B. Van Hasselt, New York, Plenum Press.
  409. SHEVRIN H. (2001). Event-related markers of unconscious processes. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 209-218.
  410. SHEVRIN H. (2003). T he dream interpreters: a psychoanalytic novel in verse. York: International Universities Press, 2003, 375 p.
  411. SHEVRIN H., BOND J., BRAKEL L., HERTEL R. & WILLIAMS W. (1996). Conscious and unconscious processes : psychodynamic, cognitive and neuro-physiologic convergences. New York, Guilford Press.
  412. SHEVRIN H. & FISHER C. (1967). Changes in the effects of a waking subliminal stimulus as a function of dreaming and non-dreaming sleep. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 4, 362-368.
  413. SHEVRIN H. & FRITZLER D. (1968). Visual evoked response correlates of unconscious mental processes. Science, 161, 295-298.
  414. SHEVRIN H., GHANNAM J.H. & LIBET B. (2002a). A neural correlate of consciousness related to repression. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 334-341.
  415. SHEVRIN H., GHANNAM J.H. & LIBET B. (2002b). Response to commentary on “A neural correlate of consciousness related to repression”. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 345-346.
  416. SHEVRIN H. & LUBORSKY L. (1958a). The measurement of preconscious perception in dreams and images: An investigation of the Poetzl phenomenon. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56, 285-294.
  417. SHEVRIN H. & LUBORSKY L. (1958b). A supplemental study of visual sources of dreams and waking images. American Psychologist, 13, 354 (a).
  418. SHEVRIN H. & LUBORSKY L. (1961). The rebus technique: A method for studying primary-process transformations of briefly exposed pictures. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 133, 479-488.
  419. SHEVRIN H., SMITH W.H. & FRITZLER D. (1969). Repressiveness as a factor in the subliminal activation of brain and verbal responses. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 149, 261-269.
  420. SHEVRIN H., SMITH W.H. & FRITZLER D. (1970). Subliminally stimulated brain and verbal responses of twins differing in repressiveness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 76, 39-46.
  421. SHEVRIN H., SMITH W.H. & FRITZLER D. (1971). Average evoked response and verbal correlates of unconscious mental processes. Psychophysiology, 6, 2, 149-162.
  422. SHEVRIN H., WILLIAMS W.J., MARSHALL R.E., HERTEL R.K., BOND J.A. & BRAKEL L.A. (1992). Event-related potential indicators of the dynamic unconscious. Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 340-366.
  423. SHULMAN R.G. & REISER M.F. (2004). Freud’s theory of mind and functional imaging experiments. Neuro-Psychoanalysis, 6, 2, 133-164.
  424. SILBERSWEIG D.A., STERN E., FRITH C.D., CAHILL C., HOLMES A., GROOTOONK S., SEEWARD J., MCKENNA P., CHUA S.E., SCHNOOR L., JONES T. & FRACKOWIAK R.S.J. (1995). A functional neuroanatomy of hallucinations in schizophrenia. Nature, 378, 176-179. (1995). A functional neuroanatomy of hallucinations in schizophrenia. Nature, 378, 176-179.
  425. SIMPSON G.B. & BURGESS C. (1985). Activation and selection processes in the recognition of ambiguous words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,11, 28-39.
  426. SIMPSON G.B. & KANG H. (1994). Inhibitory processes in the recognition of homograph meaning. Dans Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language, dir. D. Dagenbach et T.H. Carr, San Diego, Academic Press, 359-381.
  427. SIMPSON G.B. & KELLAS G. (1989). Dynamic contextual processes and lexical access. Dans Resolving Semantic Ambiguity, dir. D.S. Gorfein, New York, Springer, 41–56.
  428. SIRIGU A., DAPRATI E., CIANCIA S., GIRAUX P., NIGHOGHOSSIAN N., POSADA A. & HAGGARD P. (2004). Altered awareness of voluntary action after damage to the parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 80-84.
  429. SKIPPER J.I., VAN WASSENHOVE V., NUSBAUM H.C. & SMALL S.L. (2007). Hearing lips and seeing voices: how cortical areas supporting speech production mediate audiovisual speech perception. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2387-2399.
  430. SKOYLES J.R. (1998). Speech phones are a replication code. Medical Hypotheses, 50, 167-173.
  431. SMITH E.E., JONIDES J., MARSHUETZ C. & KOEPPE R. A. (1998). Components of verbal working memory: evidence from neuroimaging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 876-882.
  432. SNODGRASS M., BERNAT E. & SHEVRIN H. (2004). Unconscious perception: a model-based approach to method and evidence. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(5),846–867.
  433. SNODGRASS M. & SHEVRIN H. (2006). Unconscious inhibition and facilitation at the objective detection threshold: replicable and qualitatively different unconscious perceptual effects. Cognition, 101, 43–79.
  434. SNODGRASS M., SHEVRIN H. & KOPKA M. (1993). The mediation of intentional judgments by unconscious perceptions: the influences of task strategy, task preference, word meaning and motivation. Consciousness and Cognition, 2, 169-193.
  435. SO C.W., COPPOLA M., LICCIARDELLO V. & GOLDIN-MEADOW S. (2005). The Seeds of Spatial Grammar in the Manual Modality. Cognitive Science, 29, 1029-1043.
  436. SOLMS M. (1993). Summary and discussion of the paper: “The neuropsychological organization of dreaming: implications for psychoanalysis”, Bulletin of the Anna Freud Centre, 16, 149-165.
  437. SOLMS M. (1997). The neuropsychology of dreams: a clinico-anatomical study. Hillsdale, Lawrence Erlbaum.
  438. SOLMS M. (1998). Preliminaries for an integration of psychoanalysis and neuroscience. Bulletin of the British Psycho-Analytic Society, 34, 9, 23-38.
  439. SOLMS M. (2000). Introductory essay for a Centenary reprint of the 1st edition of Freud’s “Traumdeutung”, Fischer Verlag, Frankfurt am Main.
  440. SOLMS M. (2002). An introduction to the neuroscientific works of Freud. Dans The pre-psychoanalytic writings of Sigmund Freud, éds. G. Van de Vijver et F. Geerardyn, London, Karnac, pp. 25-26.
  441. SOLMS M. (2004). Freud returns. Scientific American, 5, 852-88.
  442. SOLMS M. & SALING M. (1986). On psychoanalysis and neuroscience: Freud’s attitude to the localizationist tradition. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 67, 397-416.
  443. SOLMSM. & SALING M. (1990). A moment of transition: two neuroscientific articles by Sigmund Freud, London, Karnac.
  444. SOLMS M. & TURNBULL O. (2002). The brain and the inner world: an introduction to the neuroscience of subjective experience, New York, Other Press.
  445. SPERRY R.W. (1950). Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 482-489.
  446. SPITZER M. & KAMMER T. (1996). Combining neuroscience research methods in psychopathology. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 9, 352-363.
  447. SPITZER M., HERMLE L. & MAIER S. (1993). Associative semantic network dysfunction in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients: direct evidence from indirect semantic priming. Biological Psychiatry, 34, 864-877.
  448. SQUIRE L.R. & KANDEL E. (1999). Memory: from mind to molecules, New York, Scientific American Library; trad. franç., Bruxelles, De Boeck, 2002.
  449. STANOVICH K.E. & WEST R.F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning : Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 645–726.
  450. STEPHAN K.M., FINK G.R., PASSINGHAM R.E., FRITH C.D. & FRACKOWIAK, R.S.J. (1995). Functional anatomy of the mental representation of hand movements in healthy subjects. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 373-386.
  451. STORA J.-B. (1999). Quand le corps prend la relève, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  452. STORA J.-B. (2006). La neuro-psychanalyse, Collection « Que sais-je?», Paris, PUF.
  453. STREMLER E. & CASTEL P-H. (2009). Pour une histoire des débuts de la neuropsychanalyse : premiers éléments de réflexion à partir de sources inédites. Dans Vers une neuropsychanalyse?, ed. L. Ouss et B. Golse, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  454. STUDDERT-KENNEDY M. (1991). Language development from an evolutionary perspective. Dans Biological and behavioral determinants of language development, dir. Norman A. Krasnegor et al., Hillsdale (NJ), Erlbaum, 5-28.
  455. STUDDERT-KENNEDY M. (2000). Imitation and the Emergence of Segments. Phonetica, 57, 2-4.
  456. STUDDERT-KENNEDY M. & GOODELL E. (1995). Gestures, features and segments in early child speech. Dans Speech and reading: a comparative approach, dir.B. de Gelder et J. Morais, East Sussex, Erlbaum, 65-85.
  457. SULLOWAY F.J. (1979). Freud, biologist of the mind. Beyond the psychoanalytic legend. New York, Basic Books.
  458. SWICK D. & KNIGHT R.T. (1996). Contributions of prefrontal cortex to recognition memory: electrophysiological and behavioral evidence. Neuropsychology, 13, 155-170.
  459. SWINNEY D.A. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 18, 6, 645-659.
  460. TANENHAUS M.K. & LUCAS M.M. (1987). Context effects in lexical processing. Cognition, 25, 213-234.
  461. TANENHAUS M.K., LEIMAN J.M. & SEIDENBERG M.S. (1979). Evidence for multiple stages in the processing of ambiguous words in syntactic contexts. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 427-440.
  462. TASSIN J.P. (1989). Peut-on trouver un lien entre l’inconscient psychanalytique et les connaissances actuelles en neurobiologie. Neuropsy, 4, 8, 421-434.
  463. TASSIN J.-P. (1991). Biologie et inconscient. Dans Le cerveau dans tous ses états , Entretiens de M. Sicard, Presses du CNRS.
  464. TASSIN J.P. (1998). Drogues, plaisir et douleur. La Recherche, 306.
  465. TASSIN J.P. (2001). A quel moment survient le rêve au cours d'une nuit de sommeil? (Le rêve naît du réveil). Dans Journal de la Psychanalyse de l'Enfant : Le Rêve, 28, 82-94.
  466. TASSIN J.P. (2002). La neuropharmacologie de la conscience. Pour la science, 302, 146-150.
  467. THOMPSON-SCHILL S.L., D’ESPOSITO M., AGUIRRE G.K. & FARAH M.J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94, 14792-14797.
  468. THORPE S.J. & FABRE-THORPE M. (2001). Seeking categories in the brain. Science, 291, 260-263.
  469. TILL E.E., MROSS E.F. & KINTSCH W. (1988). Time course of priming for associate and inference words in a discourse context. Memory & Cognition, 16, 283-298.
  470. TOMPKINS C.A., BAUMGAERTNER A., LEHMAN M.T. & FASSBINDER W. (2000). Mechanisms of discourse comprehension impairment after right hemisphere brain damage: suppression in lexical ambiguity resolution. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 62-78.
  471. TRANEL D., DAMASIO H. & DAMASIO A.R. (1997). A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1319-1327.
  472. TSONIS A.A. (1992). Chaos: from theory to applications, New York, Plenum Press.
  473. TURNBULL O.H. & SOLMS M. (2007). Awareness, desire, and false beliefs: Freud in the light of modern neuropsychology, Cortex, 43, 1083-1090.
  474. TURNBULL O.H., BERRY H. & EVANS C.E.Y. (2004). A positive emotional bias in confabulatory false beliefs about place, Brain and Cognition, 55, 490-494.
  475. TURNBULL O.H., EVANS C.E.Y., BUNCE A, CARZOLIO B. & O’CONNOR J. (2005). Emotion-based learning and central executive resources: an investigation of intuition and the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition, 57, 244-247.
  476. TURNBULL O.H., JONES K. & REED-SCREEN J. (2002). Implicit awareness of deficit in anosognosia: an emotion-based account of denial of deficit. Neuropsychoanalysis, 4, 69-86;
  477. TURNBULL O.H., OWEN V. & EVANS C.E.Y. (2005). Negative emotions in anosognosia. Cortex 41, 67-75.
  478. UNGERLEIDER L.G. & MISHKIN M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. Dans Analysis of Visual Behaviour, dir. D.J. Ingle, M.A. Goodale et R. J.W. Mansfield, Cambridge, MIT Press, 549–586.
  479. URANOVA N.A., VOSTRIKOV V.M., ORLOVSKAYA D.D. & RACHMANOVA V.I. (2004). Oligoden droglial density in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia and mood disorders: a study from the Stanley Neuropathology Consortium. Schizophrenia Research, 67, 269-275.
  480. VAN BUNDER D., KNOCKAERT V., BAZAN A., VAN DE VIJVER G. & GEERARDYN F. (2002). Some remarks on the organization of human speech: the unconscious structured as a language, communication présentée lors du «First Annual International Conference on Unconscious Evolution and Cognition», Portland, Maine. 23-24 août 2002.
  481. VAN BUNDER D., KNOCKAERT V., VAN DE VIJVER G., GEERARDYN F. & BAZAN A. (2002). The return of the repressed, anticipation and the logic of the signifier. Casys. International Journal of Computing Anticipatory Systems, 12, 293-301.
  482. VAN DE VIJVER G. (1999). Du corps à l'esprit? Une analyse du matérialisme freudien. Dans Matière pensante, ed. J.-N. Missa, Paris, Vrin, 99-118.
  483. VAN DE VIJVER G. (2000). Identification and psychic closure, a dynamic structuralist approach of the psyche. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 901, 1-12.
  484. VAN DE VIJVER G., BAZAN A., ROTTIERS F. & GILBERT J. (2006) Enactivisme et internalisme: de l'ontologie à la clinique. Intellectica, 43/1, 93-103.
  485. VAN DE VIJVER G. & GEERARDYN F. (2002). The pre-psychoanalytic writings of Sigmund Freud, London, Karnac Books.
  486. VAN DE VIJVER G., VAN SPEYBROECK L., DE WAELE D., DE PREESTER H. & KOLEN F. (2005). Current philosophy of biology: outline of a transcendental project. Acta Biotheoretica, 53, 57-75.
  487. VAN HOLST E. (1954). Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. British Journal of Animal Behavior, 2, 89-94.
  488. VINCENT J.-D. (1986). Biologie des passions. Paris, Odile Jacob.
  489. VINCENT J.D. (1992). Biologie et Psychanalyse. Pour la Science, 181.
  490. VOGEL E.K. & LUCK S.J. (2000).The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. Psychophysiology, 37, 190-203.
  491. VON HELMHOLTZ H. (1867/1910). Handbuch der physiologischen optik, Dritter Band, Leipzig: Leopold Voss.
  492. VON HELMHOLTZ H. (1971). The facts of perception. Dans Selected writings of Hermann von Helmholtz, dir. R. Kahl, Middletown (CT), Wesleyan University Press.
  493. WIDLÖCHER D. (1994). Traité de psychopathologie, Paris, PUF.
  494. WIDLÖCHER D. (1996). Les nouvelles cartes de la psychanalyse, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  495. WIDLÖCHER D. (1990). Neurobiologie et psychanalyse. Les opérateurs de communication. Revue Internationale de Psychopathologie, 2, 335-356.
  496. WIDLÖCHER D. (2005). Suffit-il de bannir le mot « neuropsychanalyse? Carnet Psy, 98, éditorial.
  497. WIDLÖCHER D. (2006). Inconscient psychanalytique et neurosciences. Dans Le dictionnaire des sciences humaines, dir. S. Mesure et P. Savidan, Paris: PUF.
  498. WISE R., CHOLLET F., HADAR U., FRISTON K., HOFFNER E. & FRACKOWIAK R. (1991). Distribution of cortical neural networks involved in word comprehension and word retrieval. Brain, 114, 1803-1817.
  499. WOLPERT D.M. (1997). Computational approaches to motor control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 209–216.
  500. WOLPERT D.M., MIALL R.C. & KAWATO M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 338-347.
  501. WONG P.S., BERNAT E., BUNCE S. & SHEVRIN H. (1997). Brain indices of nonconscious associative learning. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 519-544.
  502. WONG P.S., BERNAT E., BUNCE S. & SHEVRIN H. (1999). Further evidence for unconscious learning: preliminary support for the conditioning of facial EMG to subliminal stimuli. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 33(4), 341-347.
  503. ZATORRE R., EVANS A., MEYER E. & GJEDDE A.. (1992). Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing. Science, 256, 846-849.
  504. ZATORRE R., MEYER E., GJEDDE A. & EVANS A. (1996). Pet studies of phonetic processing of speech: review, replication and reanalysis. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 21-30.
  505. ZÉNONI A. (1991). Le corps de l’être parlant, de l’évolutionnisme à la psychanalyse, Bruxelles, De Boeck, p. 31
  506. ZELKOWICZ B.J., HERBSTER A.N., NEBES R.D., MINTUN M.A. & BECKER J.T. (1997). An examination of regional cerebral blood flow during object naming tasks. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4, 160-166.