Partie II –
Les mécanismes d’attribution d’intentions chez le sujet sain et le patient schizophrène

  1. Abu-Akel, A., & Bailey, A.L. (2000). The possibility of different forms of theory of mind, Psychological Medicine, 30, 735-738.
  2. Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 1109-16.
  3. Allison, T., Puce, A., McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn Sci 4, 267–278.
  4. Amodio, D.M. & Frith, C.D. (2006). Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7, 268-77. Review.
  5. André, J.B., & Day, T. (2007). Perfect reciprocity is the only evolutionarily stable strategy in the continuous iterated prisoner's dilemma. J Theor Biol, 247, 11-22.
  6. Axelrod, R. (1997). The Complexity of Cooperation: Agent-Based Models of Competition. Princeton, Princeton University Press.
  7. Baker, C. L., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Saxe, R. R. (2006). Bayesian Models of Human Action Understanding. In Y. Weiss, B. Scholkopf, & J. Platt (Eds.), Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (Vol. 18, pp. 99-106), MIT Press.
  8. Baker, C. L., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Saxe, R. R. (2007). Goal Inference as Inverse Planning, In Proceedings of the Twenty-Ninth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
  9. Barbalat G., Chambon V., Franck N., Koechlin E., Farrer C. (2009). Organization of cognitive control within lateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 1-10.
  10. Barnes, C.L. & Pandya, D.N. (1992). Efferent cortical connections of multimodal cortex of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 318: 222-44.
  11. Baron-Cohen, S. et al. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’ ? Cognition, 21,37-46.
  12. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). « The autistic child’s theory of mind : a case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 30 295-297.
  13. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. An essay on autism and theory of mind. Trad.fr. La cécité mentale. Presses Universitaires de Grenoble, 1998.
  14. Becchio, C., Sartori, L., Bulgheroni, M., & Castiello, U. (2008). The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: A kinematic study on social intention, Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 557-64.
  15. Beckmann, M., Johansen-Berg, H., & Rushworth, M.F.S (2009). Connectivity-based parcellisation of human cingulated cortex and its relation to functional specialization. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1175-1190.
  16. Bedell, J., Lennox, S.S., Smith, A.D., & Rabinowicz, E.F. (1998). Evaluation of problem solving and communication skills of persons with schizophrenia, Psychiatry Res., 78, 197-206.
  17. Blakemore, S. J., & Decéty, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 561-567.
  18. Blakemore, S.J., Boyer, P., Pachot-Clouard, M., Meltzoff, A., Segebarth, C., & Decety, J. (2003). The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain, Cerebral cortex, 13, 837-844.
  19. Blakemore S. J., & Frith, U. (2004). How does the brain deal with the social world? Neuroreport, 15, 119-28.
  20. Blakemore, S-J. et al. (2004). Social cognitive neuroscience: where are we heading? Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 216-221.
  21. Bonda E, Petrides M, Ostry D, Evans A. (1996). Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion. J Neurosci 16: 3737-44.
  22. Brankovic, S.B., & Paunovic, V.R. (1999). Reasoning under uncertainty in deluded schizophrenic patients: A longitudinal study, European Psychiatry, 14, 76-83.
  23. Brass, M., Schmitt, R. M., Spengler, S., & Gergely, G. (2007). Investigating action understanding: inferential processes vs. action simulation. Current Biology, 17, 2117-2121.
  24. Bratman, M. E. (1987). Intention, Plans, and Practical Reason, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  25. Brunet, E. Sarfati, Y., & Hardy-Bayle, M.C. (2003). Reasoning about physical causality and other’s intentions in schizophrenia, Cogn Neuropsychiatry, 8, 129-139.
  26. Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G.R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R.J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., Freund, H.J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 400–404.
  27. Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual Change in Childhood, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  28. Carruthers, P. (1996). Simulation and self-knowledge: a defence of the theory-theory. In P. Carruthers & P.K. Smith, Eds. Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  29. Castelli, F., Happé, F., Frith, U. & Frith, C. D. (2000). Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage, 12, 314-325.
  30. Ciaramidaro, A., Adenzato, M., Enrici, I., Erk, S., Pia, L., Bara, B. G., & Walter, H. (2007). The intentional network: how the brain reads varieties of intentions. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3105-13.
  31. Colbert, S.M., & Peters, E.R. (2002). Need for closure and jumping-to-conclusions in delusion-prone individuals, Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 27-31.
  32. Corcoran, R. et al. (1995). Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: Investigating ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 17, 5–13.
  33. Corcoran, R. (2000). Theory of mind in other clinical populations. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed.), pp. 391–421. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  34. Corcoran, R. (2001). Theory of Mind in Schizophrenia, In Penn, D., Corrigan, P. (eds), Social Cognition in Schizophrenia, APA, Washington.
  35. Corcoran, R. (2001). Theory of mind in schizophrenia. In D. Penn & P. Corrigan (eds.), Social cognition in schizophrenia (pp. 149–174). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
  36. Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Bíró, S., Koós, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of 'pure reason' in infancy. Cognition, 26, 237-67.
  37. Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2007). 'Obsessed with goals': functions and mechanisms of teleological interpretation of actions in humans. Acta Psychol, 124, 60-78.
  38. Cutting, J. & Murphy, D. (1990). Impaired ability of schizophrenic, relative to manics or depressives, to appreciate social knowledge about their culture, Br. J. Psychiatry, 157, 355-358.
  39. de Lange, F. P., Spronk, M., Willems, R. M., Toni, I., & Bekkering H (2008). Complementary systems for understanding action intentions. Current Biology, 25, 454-7.
  40. Domenech, P. & Dreher J.-C. (en revision). Decision threshold in the human brain
  41. Doody, G.A. et al.(1998). Theory of mind and psychoses. Psychological Medicine, 28, 397–405.
  42. Drury, V.M. et al. (1998). ‘Theory of mind’ skills during an acute episode of psychosis and following recovery. Psychological Medicine, 28, 1101–1112.
  43. Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., & D'Ausilio, A. (2009). Broca’s area in language, action, and music. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1169, 448-58.
  44. Faillenot I, Toni I, Decety J, Grégoire MC, Jeannerod M. (1997). Visual pathways for object-oriented action and object recognition: functional anatomy with PET. Cereb Cortex 7: 77-85.
  45. Fazio P, Cantagallo A, Craighero L, D'Ausilio A, Roy AC, Pozzo T, Calzolari F, Granieri E, Fadiga L (2009). Encoding of human action in Broca’s area. Brain 132, 1980-8.
  46. Filimon, F., Nelson, J.D., Hagler, D.J., Sereno, M.I. (2007). Human cortical representations for reaching: mirror neurons for execution, observation, and imagery. Neuroimage 37, 1315–1328.
  47. Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J Pers Soc Psychol, 82, 878-902.
  48. Fletcher, P.C. & Frith, C.D. (2009). Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10: 48-58.
  49. Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P.F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., Rizzolatti, G., 2005. Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding. Science 308, 662–667.
  50. Franck, N., Farrer, C., Georgieff, N., Marie-Cardine, M., Daléry, J., d’Amato, T., & Jeannerod; M. (2001). Defective recognition of one’s own actions in patients with schizophrenia, American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 454-459. 
  51. Friston, K., Kilner, J., & Harrison, L. (2006). A free energy principle for the brain. J Physiol Paris, 100:70-87.
  52. Frith U. (1989). Autism : explaining the enigma. Trad. fr. L’énigme de l’autisme, O. Jacob, Paris, 1992.
  53. Frith, C.D. (1992). The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  54. Frith, C., & Corcoran, R. (1996). Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 26, 521–530.
  55. Frith, U. et Happé, F. (1999), Theory of Mind and Self-Consciousness: What Is It Like To Be Autistic? Mind and Language, 14 (1), pp. 1-22.
  56. Frith, U. & Frith C.D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. In The Neuroscience of Social Interaction (Frith, C. and Wolpert, D. eds.), pp. 45-75, Oxford University Press.
  57. Frith, C.D. (2004). Schizophrenia and theory of mind, Psychological Medicine, 34, 385-389.
  58. Frith, C.D. (2005). The self in action: Lessons from delusions of control, Conscious Cognitive, 14, 752-770.
  59. Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). How we predict what other people are going to do. Brain Research, 1079, 36-46.
  60. Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2007). Social cognition in humans. Current Biology, 17, 724-32.
  61. Fu, G., & Lee, K. (2007). Social grooming in the kindergarten: the emergence of flattery behavior. Developmental Science,10, 255-265.
  62. Gallagher, H.L., Frith, C.D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends Cogn Sci 7, 77–83.
  63. Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 596-609.
  64. Gallese, V., & Goldmann, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends Cogn Sci, 2, 493-501.
  65. Gallese, V. (2003). The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. Philos. Trans. R.Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 258, 517-528.
  66. Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci, 8, 396-403.
  67. Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., Keysers, C. (2007b). The anthropomorphic brain: the mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. Neuroimage 35, 1674–1684.
  68. Georgieff, N. (2000) Neuropsychopathologie cognitive sociale de l'action : apport à l'étude des symptômes positifs de la schizophrénie. Intellectica 2, 31, 191-225.
  69. Georgiou, I., Becchio, C., Glover, S., & Castiello, U. (2007). Different action patterns for cooperative and competitive behaviour. Cognition, 102, 415-433.
  70. Gergely, G., et al. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, Vol. 56, No. 2., 165-193.
  71. German, T.P., Niehaus, J.L., Meghan, P., Roarty, M.P., Giesbrecht, B., Miller, M.B., 2004. Neural correlates of detecting pretense: automatic engagement of the intentional stance under covert conditions. J. Neurosci. 16, 1805–1817.
  72. Gold, J. I., & Shadlen, M. N. (2002). Banburismus and the brain: decoding the relationship between sensory stimuli, decisions, and reward. Neuron, 36, 299-308.
  73. Goldman, A.I. (1995). In defense of the Simulation Theory. Dans Mental simulation: Philosophical and Psychological Essays, édité par M. Davies et R. Stone, Oxford : Blackwell.
  74. Goldman, A. I., & Sebanz, N. (2005). Simulation, mirroring, and a different argument from error. Trends Cogn Sci, 9, 320.
  75. Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. (1992). Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language, 7, 145-71.
  76. Gopnik, A. (1993). How we know our minds : the illusion of first-person knowledge of intentionality. The Behavioral and Brain sciences, 16, pp. 1-14.
  77. Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. (1994). The theory theory. In Hirschfeld, L. and Gelman, S. (eds.) Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  78. Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A.N. (1994). Minds, bodies and persons: Young children's understanding of the self and others as reflected in imitation and ‘theory of mind’ research. In Self-awareness in animals and humans, édité par S. Parker et R. Mitchell, New York: Cambridge University Press.
  79. Gordon, R.M. (1996). Radical simulationism. Dans Theories of Theories of Mind, édité par P. Carruthers et P. Smith, Cambridge : Cambridge University Press, pp. 11-21.
  80. Grafton, S. T., Arbib, M. A., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Localization of grasp representation in humans by positron emission tomography: 2. Observation compared with imagination. Experimental Brain Research, 112, 103-111.
  81. Grafton, S. T., & Hamilton, A. F. (2007). Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. Hum Mov Sci, 26, 590-616.
  82. Grèzes, J., & Decéty, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 1-19.
  83. Grèzes, J., Armony, J.L., Rowe, J., & Passingham, R.E(2003). Activations related to ‘mirror’ and ‘canonical’ neurones in the human brain: an fRMI study. NeuroImage 18, 928–937.
  84. Griffiths, T. L., Kemp, C., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2008). Bayesian models of cognition, In Ron Sun (ed.), The Cambridge handbook of computational cognitive modelling, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  85. Happé, F.G.E. (1995). The role of verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843-855.
  86. Hamilton, A.C. & Grafton, S. (2006). Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci 26, 1133–1137.
  87. Hamilton, A.C. & Grafton, S. (2008). Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1160-1168.
  88. Harrington, L., Siegert, R. J. & McClure, J. (2005) Theory of mind in schizophrenia: a critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10, 249 -286.
  89. Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behaviour. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243-259
  90. Herold, R. et al. (2002). Theory of mind deficit in people with schizophrenia during remission. Psychological Medicine, 32, 1125–1129.
  91. Hugues, C. et al. (1984). Evidence for central executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477-492.
  92. Huq, S.F., Garety, P.A., & Hemsley, D.R. (1988). Probabilistic judgements in deluded and non-deluded subjects, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 40, 801-812.
  93. Hurlburt, R.T. et al.(1994), Sampling the form of inner experience in three adults with Asperger syndrome. Psychol Med, 24(2), pp. 385-95.
  94. Iacoboni, M. et al. (1999). Cortical mechanism of human imitation. Science 286, 2526-2528.
  95. Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J.C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3, 529-535.
  96. Jabbi, M., Bastiaansen, J., & Keysers, C. (2008). A common anterior insula representation of disgust observation, experience and imagination shows divergent functional connectivity pathways. PLoS One, 13, e2939.
  97. Jacob, P. (2008). What do mirror neurons contribute to human social cognition? Mind and Language, 23, 190-223.
  98. Jacob, P. (2009). The tuning-fork model of human social cognition: a critique. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 229-43.
  99. Jacob, P., & Jeannerod, M. (2005). The motor theory of social cognition: a critique. Trends Cogn Sci, 9, 21-25.
  100. Jeannerod, M. (1995). Mental imagery in the motor context. Neuropsychologia, 33.
  101. Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage, 14, 103-109.
  102. Jeannerod, M., & Pacherie, E. (2004). Agency, simulation, and self-identification. Mind and Language, 19, 113-146.
  103. Jones, E. (1999). The phenomenology of abnormal belief. Philosophy, Psychiatry and Psychology, 6, 1-16.
  104. Kaliuzhna, M., Chambon, V., & van der Henst, J.-B. (submitted). Investigating hypothesis testing and advice taking in deluded patients: should we revise our beliefs about belief revision?
  105. Koechlin, E., Danek, A., Burnod, Y., and Grafman, J. (2002). Medial prefrontaland subcortical mechanisms underlying the acquisition of motor and cognitiveaction sequences in humans. Neuron 35, 371–381
  106. Kouneiher, F., Charron, S., & Koechlin, E. (2009). Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 939-947.
  107. Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2007). Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci, 11, 194-6.
  108. Keysers, C. & Perrett, D.I. (2004). Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci 8, 501–507.
  109. Kilner JM, Vargas C, Duval S, Blakemore SJ, Sirigu A. (2004). Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement. Nat Neurosci 7: 1299-301.
  110. Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007a). Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system. Cogn Process, 8, 159-66.
  111. Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007b). The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. Neuroreport, 18, 619-23.
  112. King-Casas, B., Tomlin, D., Anen, C., Camerer, C. F., Quartz, S. R., & Montague, P. R. (2005). Getting to know you: reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange.Science, 308, 78-83.
  113. Knill, D.C. & Pouget, A. (2004). The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation, Trends Neurosci 27: 712-9.
  114. Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2008). Evolving intentions for social interaction: from entrainment to joint action. Philos Trans R Soc B, 363, 2021-31.
  115. Körding, K. P., & Wolpert, D. M. (2006). Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control. Trends Cogn Sci, 10, 319-26.
  116. Langdon, R., Coltheart, M., Ward, P.B., & Catts, S.V. (2002). Disturbed communication in schizophrenia: The role of poor pragmatics and poor mind-reading, Psychological Medicine, 32, 1273-1284.
  117. Lee, T.W., Josephs, O., Dolan, R.J., & Critchley, H.D. (2006). Imitating expressions: emotion-specific neural substrates in facial mimicry. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci, 1, 122-135.
  118. Leekam, S.R., & Prior, M. (1994). Can autistic children distinguish lies from jokes? A second look at second-order belief attribution. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 901-915.
  119. Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of “theory of mind”. Psychologica l Review, 94, 412-42.
  120. Leslie, A.M., & Polizzi, P. (1998). Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: Twoconjectures. Developmental Science, 1, 247–254.
  121. Luppino G, Murata A, Govoni P, Matelli M. (1999). Largely segregated parietofrontal connections linking rostral intraparietal cortex (areas AIP and VIP) and the ventral premotor cortex (areas F5 and F4). Exp Brain Res 128:181-7.
  122. Maguire E.A. (2001) The retrosplenial contribution to human navigation: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. Scand J Psychol 42:225–238.
  123. Mamassian, P., & Goutcher, R. (2001). Prior knowledge on the illumination position. Cognition, 81, B1-9.
  124. McCabe, R. (2004). On the Inadequacies of Theory of Mind Explanations of Schizophrenia. Alternative Accounts of Alternative Problems, Theory & Psychology, 14,738-752.
  125. Manto, M. (2009). Mechanisms of human cerebellar dysmetria: experimental evidence and current conceptual bases. J Neuroeng Rehabil, 13, 6-10. Review.
  126. Mazza, M., De Risio, A., Surian, L., Roncone, R., & Casacchia, M. (2001). Selective impairment of theory of mind in people with schizophrenia, Schizophrenia Research, 47, 299–308.
  127. Mele, A. (1992). Springs of Action: Understanding Intentional Behavior, Oxford University Press.
  128. Metzinger, T. & Gallese, V. (2003). The emergence of a shared action ontology: building blocks for a theory. Self and Action. Special issue of Consciousness & Cognition (12:4), 549-571.
  129. Mitchell,J. P. (2005). The false dichotomy between simulation and theory-theory: the argument's error. Trends Cogn Sci, 9, 363-364.
  130. Morecraft RJ, Cipolloni PB, Stilwell-Morecraft KS, Gedney MT, Pandya DN. (2004). Cytoarchitecture and cortical connections of the posterior cingulate and adjacent somatosensory fields in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol 26: 37-69.
  131. Nachmias, J. (1981). On the psychometric function for contrast detection. Vision Research, 21, 215-223.
  132. Nichols, S., & Stich, S. (2003). Mindreading: an integrated account of pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds, Oxford University Press, New York.
  133. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113.
  134. Onishi, K.H. & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs? Science, 308: 255-8. 
  135. Onishi, K. H., Baillargeon, R., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). 15-month-old infants detect violations in pretend scenarios. A cta Psychol, 124, 106-28.
  136. Oztop, E., Wolpert, D., & Kawato, M. (2005). Mental state inference using visual control parameters. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 129-151.
  137. Pacherie, E. (2000). The content of intentions. Mind and Language, 15, 400-432.
  138. Pacherie, E. (2008). The phenomenology of action: a conceptual framework. Cognition, 107, 179-217.
  139. Pelphrey KA, Mitchell TV, McKeown MJ, Goldstein J, Allison T, McCarthy G. (2003). Brain activity evoked by the perception of human walking: controlling for meaningful coherent motion. J Neurosci 23: 6819-25.
  140. Perner, J. et al. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child’s « theory of mind »: knowledge, belief and communication. Child Development, 60, 689-700.
  141. Phillips, W. (1993). Understanding intention and desire by children with autism. Thèse de doctorat non publiée, Institute of Psychiatry, University of London.
  142. Phillips, M.L. et al. (1997). A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature 289, 495-498
  143. Picard, N. & Strick, P.L. (1996). Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. Cereb Cortex 6:342–353.
  144. Picard, N. & Strick, P.L. (2001). Imaging the premotor areas. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:663–672.
  145. Pickup, G.J. & Frith, C.D. (2001). Theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia: symptomatology, severity and specificity. Psychological Medicine 31, 207-220.
  146. Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1, 515-526.
  147. Puce, A. & Perrett, D. (2003). Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 358: 435-45.
  148. Raffman, D. (1999), What Autism May Tell Us About Self-Awareness : A Commentary on Frith and Happé. Mind and Language, 14 (1), pp. 23-31.
  149. Rapoport, A., & Chammah, A. M. (1965). Prisoner's Dilemma: A Study in Conflict and Cooperation , University of Michigan Press.
  150. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131-141.
  151. Rizzolatti G., et al. (2000). Cortical mechanism subserving object grasping and action recognition: a new view on the cortical motor functions. In The New Cognitive Neurosciences (Gazzaniga, ed.), pp. 539-552.
  152. Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat Neurosci Rev,2, 661-670.
  153. Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci, 27, 169-92.
  154. Rushworth, M.F. & Taylor, P.C. (2006). TMS in the parietal cortex: updating representations for attention and action. Neuropsychologia 44, 2700-16.
  155. Sakata H, Taira M, Murata A, Mine S. (1999). Neural mechanisms of visual guidance of hand action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. Cereb Cortex 5: 429-38.
  156. Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Bayle, M.C., Nadel, J., Chevalier, J.-FR., & Widlocher, D. (1997). Attribution of mental states to others by schizophrenic patients, Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 1-17.
  157. Sarfati, Y., Hardy-Bayle, M.C., Brunet, E., & Widlocher, D. (1999). Ivestigating theory of mind in schizophrenia: influence of verbalization in disorganized and non-disorganized patients, Schizophrenia Research, 25, 183-190.
  158. Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn Sci, 10, 70-6.
  159. Sebanz, N., & Shiffrar, M. (2009). Detecting deception in a bluffing body: the role of expertise. Psycho Bull Rev, 16, 70-5.
  160. Saxe, R. et al. (2004). Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. American Review of Psychology 55, 87-124.
  161. Saxe, R., Xiao, D.K., Kovacsc, G., Perrett, D.I., Kanwisher, N. (2004b). A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia 42, 1435–1446.
  162. Saxe, R. (2005). Against simulation: the argument from error. Trends Cogn Sci, 9, 174-9.
  163. Schneider, K. (1959). Clinical Psychopathology. New York: Grune & Stratton.
  164. Scholl, B. J., & Tremoulet, P. D. (2000). Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends Cogn Sci, 4, 299-309.
  165. Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality: An Essay in the Philosophy of Mind, Cambridge University Press.
  166. Searle, J. (1995). The Construction of Social Reality, New York: The Free Press.
  167. Seltzer B & Pandya DN (1994). Parietal, temporal, and occipital projections to cortex of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey: a retrograde tracer study. J Comp Neurol. 15: 445-63.
  168. Singer, T. et al. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not the sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157-1162
  169. Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. N ature, 439, 466-9.
  170. Spiers HJ, Maguire EA (2007) The neuroscience of remote spatial memory: a tale of two cities. Neuroscience 149:7–27.
  171. Sprong, M., Schothorst, P., Vost, E., Hox, J., & van Engeland, H. (2007). Theory of mind in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis, British Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 5-13.
  172. Summerfield, C. & Koechlin E. (2008). A neural representation of prior information during perceptual inference. Neuron, 59: 336-347.
  173. Summerfield, C. & Egner, T. (2009). Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition, Trends Cogn Sci, 13: 403-9.
  174. Surian, L., Caldi, S. & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of Beliefs by 13-Month-Old Infants. Psychological Science, 18: 580-6.
  175. Taylor, G.J. (1994). The alexithymia construct: conceptualization, validation, and relationship with basic dimensions of personality, New Trends Exp Clin Psychiatry, 10, 61–74.
  176. Tenenbaum, J.B., Griffiths, T. L., & Niyogi, S. (2007). Intuitive theories as grammars for causal inference. In Gopnik, A., & Schulz, L. (eds.), Causal learning: Psychology, philosophy, and computation. Oxford University Press.
  177. Umiltà MA, Kohler E, Gallese V, Fogassi L, Fadiga L, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G. (2001). I know what you are doing. a neurophysiological study. Neuron 31: 155-65.
  178. Van der Cruyssen, L., Van Duynslaeger, M., Cortoos, A., Van Overwalle, F. (2009). ERP time course and brain areas of spontaneous and intentional goal inferences. Soc Neurosci 4, 165–184.
  179. Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp 30, 829–858.
  180. Van Overwalle, F. & Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis. NeuroImage, in press.
  181. Walter, H., Adenzato, M., Ciaramidaro, A., Enrici, I., Pia, L., Bara, B.G. (2004). Understanding intentions in social interaction: the role of the anterior paracingulate cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1854–1863.
  182. Walter, H., Ciaramidaro, A., Adenzato, M., Vasic, N., Ardito, R.B., Erk, S., & Bara, B.G. (2009). Dysfunction of the social brain in schizophrenia is modulated by intention type: An fMRI type, SCAN, 4, 166-176.
  183. Wichmann, F. A., & Hill, N. J. (2001). The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Perception & Psychophysics,63, 1293-1313.
  184. Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representations and constraining functions of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128.
  185. Wolpert, D.M. et al. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. 358: 593-602.
  186. Ybarra, O. (2002). Naive causal understanding of valenced behaviors and its implications for social information processing. Pychol Bull, 128, 421-41.
  187. Zalla, T., Verlut, I., Franck, N., Puzenat, D., & Sirigu, A. (2004). Perception of dynamic action in patients with schizophrenia, Psychiatry Research, 128,39-51.
  188. Zalla, T., Bouchilloux, N., Labruyere, N., Georgieff, N., Bougerol, T., & Franck, N. (2006). Impairment in event sequencing in disorganised and non-disorganised patients with schizophrenia. Brain Research Bulletin, 68,195-202.