Références bibliographiques

  1. Adlard, A., & Hazan, V. (1998). Speech perception in children with specific reading difficulties (Dyslexia). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51(1), 153-177.
  2. Ahissar, M., Protopapas, A., Reid, M., & Merzenich, M. M. (2000). Auditory processing parallels reading abilities in adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97, 6832-6837.
  3. Alexander, A. W., Anderson, H. G., Heilman, P. C., Voeller, K., & Torgesen, J. (1991). Phonological awareness training and remediation of analytic decoding deficits in a group of severe dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 193–206.
  4. Alvares, C. J., Carreiras, M., & Taft, M. (2001). Syllables and morphemes: Contrasting frequency effects in Spanish. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(2), 545-555.
  5. Amitay, S., Ben-Yehudah, G., Banai, K., & Ahissar, M. (2002). Disabled readers suffer from visual and auditory impairments but not from a specific magnocellular deficit. Brain, 125, 2272-2285.
  6. Anderson, J. L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2003). A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech, 46, 155-182.
  7. Andruski, J., Blumstein, S. E., & Burton, M. (1994). The effect of subphonetic differences on lexical access. Cognition, 52, 173-187.
  8. Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review, 105(4), 678-723.
  9. Ashby, J. (2010). Phonology is fundamental in skilled reading: Evidence from ERPs. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 17(1), 95-100.
  10. Ashby, J., & Martin, A. E. (2008). Prosodic phonological representations early in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Perception, and Performance, 34(1), 224-236.
  11. Ashby, J., Sanders, L. D., & Kingston, J. (2009). Skilled readers begin processing sub-phonemic features by 80 ms during visual word recognition: Evidence from ERPs. Biological Psychology, 80, 84-94.
  12. Aslin, R. N., Pisoni, D. B., Hennessy, B. L., & Perey, A. V. (1981). Discrimination of voice onset time by human infants: New findings and implications for the effect of early experience. Child Development, 52, 1135-1145.
  13. Baddeley, A.D. (1968). How does acoustic similarity influence short-term memory? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 249-264.
  14. Baddeley, A. D. (1966). Short term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 362-365.
  15. Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158-173.
  16. Bailey, T. M., & Hahn, U. (2005). Phoneme similarity and confusability. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 339-362.
  17. Bailey, T. M., & Hahn, U. (2001). Determinants of wordlikeness: Phonotactics or lexical neighborhoods? Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 568-591.
  18. Bailey, C. E., Manis, F. R., Pederson, W. C., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Variation among developmental dyslexics: Evidence from a printed-word-learning task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(2), 125-154.
  19. Bailey, T. M., & Plunkett, K. (2002). Phonological specificity in early words. Cognitive Development, 17, 1265-1282.
  20. Baker, E., Blumstein, S.E., & Goodglass, H. (1981). Interaction between phonological and semantic factors in auditory comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 19(1), 1-15.
  21. Baldeweg, T., Richardson, A., Watkins, S., Foale, C., & Gruzelier, J. (1999). Impaired auditory frequency discrimination in dyslexia detected with mismatch evoked potentials. Annals of Neurology, 45, 495-503.
  22. Baron, J. (1973). Phonemic stage not necessary for reading. Quartely Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 241-246.
  23. Bailey, C. E., Manis, F. R., Pedersen, W. C., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Variation among developmental dyslexics: Evidence from a printed-word-learning task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(2), 125-154.
  24. Bednarek, D. B., & Grabowska, A. (2002). Luminance and chromatic contrast sensitivity in dyslexia: The magnocellular deficit hypothesis revisited. Neuroreport, 13, 2521-2525.
  25. Bednarek, D. B., Saldana, D., Quintero-Gallego, E., Garcia, I., Grabowska, A., & Gomez, C. (2004). Attentional deficit in dyslexia: A general or specific impairment. NeuroReport, 15, 1787-1790.
  26. Bedoin, N. (1995). Articulation de codages phonologiques et sémantiques en lecture silencieuse: étude du décours temporal des codes phonologiques. Revue de Phonétique Appliquée, 115, 101-117.
  27. Bedoin, N. (1998a, septembre). Phonological feature activation in visual word recognition. Paper presented at the Xth Congress of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP), Jerusalem: Israël.
  28. Bedoin, N. (1998b, novembre). Les traits phonétiques en lecture silencieuse. Conférence invitée aux IIe journées scientifiques de l’Ecole d’Orthophonie de Lyon : « A propos de la conscience phonologique », Lyon: France.
  29. Bedoin, N. (2002). Relationships between temporal aspects of speech perception and dyslexia. Paper presented at The ISCA Workshop on Temporal Integration in the Perception of Speech (TIPS), Aix-en-Provence: France.
  30. Bedoin, N. (2003). Sensitivity to voicing similarity in printed stimuli: Effect of a training programme in dyslexic children. Journal of Phonetics, 31, 541-546.
  31. Bedoin, N., & Chavand, H. (2000). Functional hemispheric asymmetry in voicing feature processing in reading. Proceedings of The Tenth Annual Meeting of the Society for Text an d Discourse (pp. 226-227), France: Lyon.
  32. Bedoin, N., & Dissard, P. (2002). Sonority and syllabic structure in reading: Differences between French and English readers. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition, 2, 67-83
  33. Bedoin, N., & Dos Santos, C. (2008). How do consonants feature values affect the processing of a CVCV structure? Evidence from a reading task. Written Language and Literacy, 11(2), 188-207.
  34. Bedoin, N., Ferragne, E., & Marsico, E. (en révision). Processing of French voiced stops: Place of articulation requires the left hemisphere; voicing requires both. Brain and Language.
  35. Bedoin, N., & Krifi, S. (2005, juin). The hierarchy of phonetic features in printed syllables matching, in adult skilled readers, normally developing young readers and dyslexic children. Conference presented at the Workshop Structure and Dynamics of the Phonetic-Phonological Systems, Lyon: France.
  36. Bedoin, N., & Krifi, S. (2008, septembre). Position des syllabes et effets de ressemblance infra-phonémique en lecture. Communication orale à la Société Française de Psychologie (SFP), Symposium Le rôle de la syllable dans la lecture en français chez le lecteur apprenant et lecteur expert, Bordeaux: France.
  37. Bedoin, N., & Krifi, S. (2009). The complexity of phonetic features organisation in reading. In F. Pellegrino, E. Marsico, I. Chitoran, & Ch. Coupé (Eds.), Approaches to phonological complexity. Berlin: Mouton & de Gruyter.
  38. Bedoin, N., Levy-Sebbag, H., & Keïta, L. (2005, septembre). Distinct cognitive impairments for hierarchical letters and hierarchical objects processing in phonological and surface developmental dyslexia. Communication affichée à XIVth Conference of the European Society of Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP 14).Leiden: Holland.
  39. Bedoin, N., Roussel, C., Leculier, L., Kéïta, L., Herbillon, V., & Launay, L. (2009). Dyslexie de surface chez l’enfant et déficit de l’inhibition des détails: aide au diagnostic et remédiation. In A. Devevy (Ed.), Dyslexies: approches thérapeutiques, de la psychologie cognitive à la linguistique. Marseille: Solal.
  40. Béland, R., Paradis, C., & Bois, M. (1993). Constraints and repairs in aphasic speech: A group study. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 38, 279-302.
  41. Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: Comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6, 183-200.
  42. Benki, J. R. (2003). Analysis of English nonsense syllable recognition in noise. Phonetica, 60, 129-157.
  43. Berent, I. (1997). Phonological priming in the lexical decision task: Regularity effects are not necessary evidence for assembly. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1727-1742.
  44. Berent, I., & Perfetti, C. A. (1995). A Rose is REEZ: The two-cycles model of phonology assembly in reading English. Psychological Review, 102, 146-184.
  45. Bernhardt, B., & Stemberger, J. P. (2007). Phonological impairment in children and adults. In P. de Lacy (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology (pp. 575-593). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  46. Bertoncini, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Jusczyk, P. W., Kennedy, L. J., & Mehler, J. (1988). An investigation of young infants’ perceptual representations of speech sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(1), 21–33.
  47. Best, C. T., Lafleur, R., & Roberts, G. W. (1995). Divergent developmental patterns for infants’ perception of two non-native constrasts. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 339-350.
  48. Bijeljac-Babic, R., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1993). How do four-day-old infants categorized multisyllabic utterances? Developmental Psychology, 29, 711-721.
  49. Blumstein, S. E. (2003). Phonetic category structure and its influence on lexical processing. In A. Agwuele, W. Warren, & S.-H. Park (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2003 Texas Linguistics Society Conference (pp. 17-25). Somerville, MA : Cascadilla Proceedings Projects.
  50. Blumstein, S. E. (1990). Phonological deficits in aphasia: Theorical perspectives. In A. Caramazza (Ed.), Cognitive neuropsychology and neurolinguistics: Advances in models of cognitive function and impairment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  51. Blumstein, S. E. (1973). A phonological investigation of aphasic speech. The Hague: Mouton.
  52. Blumstein, S. E., Baker, E., & Goodglass, H. (1977). Phonological factors in auditory comprehension in aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 15(1), 19-30.
  53. Boemio, A., Fromm, S., Braun, A., & Poeppel, D. (2005). Hierarchical and asymmetric temporal sensitivity in human auditory cortices. Nature Neuroscience, 8(3), 389-395.
  54. Bogliotti, C., Messaoud-Galusi, S., & Serniclaes, W. (2002). Relations entre la perception catégorielle de la parole et l’apprentissage de la lecture. Actes des XXIVèmes Journées d’Etude sur la Parole (pp. 197-200), Nancy.
  55. Bogliotti, C., Serniclaes, W., Messaoud-Galusi, S., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2008). Discrimination of speech sounds by children with dyslexia: Comparisons with chronological age and reading level controls. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2), 137-155.
  56. Booth, J. R., McWhinney B., & Perfetti, C. A. (1999). Quick, automatic, and general activation of orthographic and phonological representations in young readers. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 3-19.
  57. Borsting, E., Ridder, W. H., Dudeck, K., Kelley, C., Matsui, L., & Motoyama, J. (1996). The presence of a magnocellular defect depends on the type of dyslexia. Vision Research, 36, 1047-1053.
  58. Bosch, L., & Sebastian-Gallés, N. (2003). Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel constrast in the first year of life. Language and Speech, 46, 217-243.
  59. Bosman, A. M. T. (1994). Reading and spelling in children and adults: Evidence for a Single-Route Model. Doctoral dissertation, University of Amsterdam.
  60. Bosman, A. M. T., & de Groot, A. M. B. (1996). Phonologic mediation is fundamental to reading: Evidence from beginning readers. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 715-744.
  61. Bosman, A. M. T., & Van Orden, G. C. (1997). Why spelling is more difficult than reading? In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp.173-194). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  62. Bosse, M. L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2006). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 19, 198-230.
  63. Bourciez, E.J. (1989). Phonétique française, étude historique. Paris: Edition Klincksieck.
  64. Bowers, P., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 69-95.
  65. Bowey, J. A. (1990). Orthographic onsets and rimes as functional units of reading. Memory and Cognition, 18(4), 419-427.
  66. Boyle, R., & Coltheart, V. (1996). Effects of irrelevant sounds on phonological coding in reading comprehension and short-term memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 398-416.
  67. Bradshaw, J. L. (1970). Phonetic homogeneity and articulatory lengthening. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 499-507.
  68. Brand, M., Rey, A., & Peereman, R. (2003). Where is the syllable priming effect in visual word recognition? Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 435-443.
  69. Breitmeyer, B.G. (1980). Unmasking visual masking: A look at the “why” behind the veil of the “how”. Psychological Review, 87(1), 52-69.
  70. Breier, J. I., Gray, L., Fletcher, J. M., Diehl, R. L., Klaas, P., Foorman, B. R., & Molis, M. R. (2001). Perception of voice and tone onset time continua in children with dyslexia with and without attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 245-270.
  71. Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Klaas, P., & Gray, L. C. (2003) Auditory temporal processing in children with specific reading disability with and without attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 46, 31-42.
  72. Broom, Y. M., & Doctor, E. A. (1995a). Developmental phonological dyslexia: A case study of the efficacity of a remediation programme. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 725-766.
  73. Broom, Y. M., & Doctor, E. A. (1995b). Developmenatl surface dyslexia: A case study of the efficacity of a remediation programme. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 69-110.
  74. Brown, W., Eliez, S., Menon, V., Rumsey, J., White, C., & Reiss, A. (2001). Preliminary evidence of widespread morphological variations in the brain in dyslexia. Neurology, 56, 781-783.
  75. Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1990). Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: The Case of Initial Consonant Clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 156-178.
  76. Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C. J., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke’s Wortschatz? Brain, 122, 1901-1917.
  77. Bryant, P. E., & Impey, L. (1986). The similarities between normal readers and developmental and acquired dyslexic children. Cognition, 24, 121-137.
  78. Brysbaert, M. (2001). Prelexical phonological coding of visual words in Dutch: Automatic after all. Memory and Cognition, 29, 765-773.
  79. Bundesen, C. (1998). Visual selective attention: Outlines of a choice model, a race model and a computational theory. Visual Cognition, 5, 287-309.
  80. Bybee, J. L., & Slobin, D.I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the english past tense. Language, 58(2), 265-289.
  81. Calliope (1989). La parole et son traitement automatique. Paris: Masson.
  82. Caplan, D., & Aydelott Utman, J. (1994). Selective acoustic phonetic impairment and lexical access in an aphasic patient. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 95, 512-517.
  83. de Cara, B. (2000). Profil de sonorité et traitement de la syllabe chez l’apprenti-lecteur (Chap. 1). Doctoral dissertation. Villeneuve d’Ascq: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.
  84. Carreiras, M., Ferrand, L., Grainger, J., & Perea, M. (2005). Sequential effects of phonological priming in visual word recognition. Psychological Science, 16(8), 585-589.
  85. Carter, D. M. (1987). An information-theoric analysis of phonetic dictionary access. Computer Speech and Language, 2, 1-11.
  86. Casalis, S. (2003). The delay-type in developmental dyslexia: Reading processes. Current Psychology Letters: Behavior, Brain and Cognition, 10. http://cpl.revues.org/document95.html.
  87. Casalis, S. (1995). Lecture et dyslexies de l’enfant. Paris: Septentrion.
  88. Casco, C., Tressoldi, P. E., & Dellantonio, A. (1998). Visual selective attention and reading efficiency are related in children. Cortex, 34, 531-546.
  89. Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Cognitive correlates of developmental surface dyslexia: A single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 25-50.
  90. Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149-180.
  91. Cestnick, L. (2001). Cross-modality temporal processing deficits in developmental phonological dyslexics. Brain and Cognition, 46, 319–325.
  92. Cestnick, L., & Coltheart, M. (1999). The relationship between language and visual processing in developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 71(3), 231-255.
  93. Cestnick, L., & Jerger, J. (2000). Auditory temporal processing and lexical/nonlexical reading in developmental dyslexia. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 11, 501–513.
  94. Charles-Luce, J., & Luce, P. A. (1995). An examination of similarity neighbourhoods in young children’s receptive vocabularies. Journal of Child Language, 22(3), 727-735.
  95. Chavand, H. (1998). Activation de traits infra-phonémiques au cours de la reconnaissance visuelle de mots. Mémoire de DEA de Psychologie Cognitive, Université Lumière Lyon 2.
  96. Chavand, H., & Bedoin, N. (1998). Phonetic features activation in visual word recognition: The case of the articulation mode. Poster presented at the Xth Congress of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP), Jerusalem: Israel.
  97. Chetail, F., & Mathey, S. (2009). Syllabic priming in lexical decision and naming tasks: The syllable congruency effect re-examined in French. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(1), 40-48.
  98. Chiappe, P., Chiappe, D. L., & Siegel, L. S. (2001). Speech perception, lexicality, and reading skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 58-74.
  99. Chiappe, P., Stringer, N., Siegel, L. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Why the timing deficit hypothesis does not explain reading disability in adults. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 73-107.
  100. Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.
  101. Clements, N. (1985). The geometry of phonological features. Phonological Yearbook, 2, 225-252.
  102. Clements, G. N., & Hume, E. W. (1995). The internal organization of speech sounds. In J. A. Goldsmith (Ed.), Handbook of phonological theory (pp. 245-306). Oxford : Blackwell.
  103. Coady, & Aslin, R. N. (2003). Phonological neighbourhoods in the developing lexicon. Journal of Child Language, 30, 441-469.
  104. Cohen, H. (1981). Hemispheric contributions to the perceptual representation of speech sounds. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Montreal: Concordia University.
  105. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (second edition). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  106. Cohen, J. D., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J. (1993). Psyscope: A new graphic interactive environment for designing psychology experiments. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 25, 257-271.
  107. Cohen, H., & Segalowitz, N. S. (1990). The role of linguistic prosody in the perception of time-compressed speech: A laterality study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 39.
  108. Cohen, H., & Segalowitz, N. S. (1990b). Cerebral hemispheric involvement in the acquisition of new phonetic categories. Brain and Language, 38, 398-409.
  109. Cohen, L., Lehericy, S., Chochon, F., Lemer, C., Rivaud, S., & Dehaene, S. (2002). Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. Brain, 125, 1054-1069.
  110. Cole, R. A. (1973). Listening for mispronunciations: A measure of what we hear during speech. Perception and Psychophysics, 1, 153-156.
  111. Cole, R. A., Jakimik, J., & Cooper, W. E. (1978). Perceptibility of phonetic features in fluent speech. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 64(1), 44-56.
  112. Colé, P., Magnan, A., & Grainger, J. (1999). Syllabe-sized units in visual words recognition: Evidence from skilled and beginning readers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 507-532.
  113. Colé, P., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (1999). Traitement syllabique au cours de la reconnaissance de mots écrits chez des enfants dyslexiques, lecteurs en retard et normo-lecteurs de 11 ans. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 9(4), 323-360.
  114. Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing. London: Academic Press.
  115. Coltheart, M., Avons, S. E., Masterson, J., & Laxon, V. J. (1991). The role of assembled phonology in reading comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 19, 387-400.
  116. Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589-608.
  117. Coltheart, M., Masterson, J., Byng, S., Prior, M., & Riddoch, J. (1983). Surface dyslexia. Quarterly Journal of Psychology, 35, 469-497.
  118. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204-256.
  119. Connine, C. M., Blasko, D. G., & Titone, D. (1993). Do the beginnings of spoken words have a special status in auditory word recognition? Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 193-210.
  120. Connine, C. M., Titone, D., Deelman, T., & Blasko, D. G. (1997). Similarity mapping in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 463-480.
  121. Conrad, R. (1972). Speech and reading. In J. F. Kavanagh & I. G. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye. The relationship between speech and reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  122. Conrad, M., Carreiras, M., Tamm, S., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Syllables and bigrams: Orthographic redundancy and syllabic units affect visual word recognition at different processing levels. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Perception, and Performance, 35(2), 461-479.
  123. Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 429-432.
  124. Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2004). Replicating syllable-freqeuncy effects in Spanish in German: One more challenge to computational models of visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Processes, 19(3), 369-390.
  125. Cornelissen, P., Richardson, A., Mason, A., Fowler, S., & Stein, J. (1995). Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measures at photopic luminance levels in dyslexic readers and controls. Vision Research, 35, 1483-1494.
  126. Csepe, V., Gyurkocza, E. E., & Osman-Sagi, J. (1998). Normal and distributed phoneme perception as reflected by the mismatch negativity: Do event-related potentials help to understand dyslexia ? Pathophysiology, 5, 202.
  127. Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1986). The syllable’s differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 385-400.
  128. Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1992). The monolingual nature of speech segmentation by bilinguals. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 381-410.
  129. Cutting, J. E. (1974). Two left-hemisphere mechanisms in speech perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 16(3), 601-612.
  130. Danon-Boileau, L., & Barbier D. (2003). Play-On, logiciel de rééducation en lecture. Université Paris V, L.A.A. Multimédias.
  131. Darwin, C. J. (1971). Ear differences in the recall of fricatives and vowels. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23(1), 46-62.
  132. de Gelder, B., & Vroomen, J. (1998). Impaired speech perception in poor readers: Evidence from hearing and speech reading. Brain and Language, 64, 269–281.
  133. Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Pallier, C., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Jobert, A., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Neural correlates of switching from auditory to speech perception. NeuroImage, 24, 21-33.
  134. Delattre, P. (1965). Comparing the phonetic features of English, French, German and Spanish: An interim report.Heidelberg: Julius Groos Verlag.
  135. Dell, G. S. (1984). The representation of serial order in speech: Evidence from the repeated phoneme effect in speech errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 222-233.
  136. Dell, G., Schwartz, M., Martin, N., Saffran, E., & Gagnon, D. (1997). Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. Psychological Review, 104, 801-838.
  137. Demb, J. B., Boynton, G. M., Best, M., & Heeger, D. J. (1998). Psychophysical evidence for a magnocellular deficit in dyslexics. Vision Research, 38, 1555-1559.
  138. Démonet, J. F., Fiez, J. A., Paulesu, E., Peterson, S. E., & Zatorre, R. J. (1996). PET studies of phonological processing. A critical reply to Poeppel. Brain and Language, 28, 352-385.
  139. Démonet, J. F., Taylor, M., & Chaix, Y. (2004). Seminar: Developmental dyslexia. Lancet, 363, 1451–1460.
  140. Démonet, J. F., Thierry, G., & Cardebat, D. (2005). Renewal of the neurophysiology of language: Functional neuroimaging. Physioogical Review, 85, 49-95.
  141. Denes, P. B. (1963). On the statistics of spoken English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 35, 892-904.
  142. Di Betta, A. M., & Romani, C. (2006). Lexical learning and dysgraphia in a group of adults with developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 376-400.
  143. Doignon, N., & Zagar, D. (2005). Illusory conjunctions in French: The nature of sublexical unities in visual word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20(3), 443-464.
  144. Dollaghan, C. A. (1994). Children’s phonological neighbourhoods: Half empty or half full ? Journal of Child Language, 21, 257-271.
  145. Dufor, O., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Démonet, J.-F. (2007). Top-down processes during auditory phoneme categorization in dyslexia: A PET study. NeuroImage, 34, 1694-1707.
  146. Dufor, O., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Démonet, J.-F. (2009). Left premotor cortex and allophonic speech perception in dyslexia: A PET study. NeuroImage, 46, 241-248.
  147. Dworczak, F., Bedoin, N., & Krifi, S. (2008). Internet et troubles d’apprentissage de la lecture: les synergies et partenariats créés. In Pithon, Asdih & Larrivée (Eds.), Construire une communauté éducative (le partenariat familles – associations – écoles – universités). Bruxelles: De Boeck Education, Collection Perspectives en Education et Formation.
  148. Eaton, T. (personal communication). Exploring the sub-phonemic level within the word recognition system.
  149. Eckert, M. A. (2004). Neuroanatomical markers for dyslexia: A Review of dyslexia structural imaging studies. The Neuroscientist, 10, 362-371.
  150. Eckert, M. A., Leonard, C. M., Richards, T. L., Aylward, E. H., Thomson, J., & Berninger, V. W. (2003). Anatomical correlates of dyslexia: Frontal and cerebellar findings. Brain, 126, 482-494.
  151. Eden, G. F., Van Mater, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., Maisog, J. M., Woods, R. P., & Zeffiro, T. A. (1996). Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature, 382, 66-69.
  152. Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Vaghoub Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250-287.
  153. Eilers, R. E., & Oller, D. K. (1976). The role of speech discrimination in developmental sound substitutions. Journal of Child Language, 3, 319-329.
  154. Eimas, P. D. (1994). Categorization in early infancy and the continuity of development. Cognition, 50, 83-93.
  155. Eimas, P. D. (1975). Distinctive feature codes in the short-term memory of children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 241-251.
  156. Eimas, P .D. (1974). Auditory and linguistic processing of cues for place of articulation by infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 513-521.
  157. Eimas, P. D., & Miller, J. L. (1980). Discrimination of information for manner of articulation. Infant Behavior and Development, 3, 367-375.
  158. Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303-306.
  159. Ellis, A. W. (1982). Spelling and writing (and reading and speaking). In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Normality and Pathology in Cognitive functions. London: Academic Press.
  160. Ellis, A. W. (1979). Slips of the pen. Visible Language, 13, 265-282.
  161. Ernestus, M., & Mak, W. M. (2004). Distinctive phonological features differ in relevance for both spoken and written word recognition. Brain and Language, 90, 378-392.
  162. Fabre, D., & Bedoin, N. (2003). Sensitivity to sonority for print processing in normal readers and dyslexic children. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition, 10, 1-6.
  163. Facoetti, A., & Molteni, M. (2001). The gradient of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 39, 352-357.
  164. Facoetti, A., Lorusso, M. L., Paganoni, P., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., & Mascetti, G. G. (2003). The time course of attentional focusing in dyslexic and normally reading children. Brain and Cognition, 53, 181-184.
  165. Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., & Lorusso, M. L. (2000a). The spatial distribution of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 531-538.
  166. Facoetti, A., Paganoni, P., Turatto, M., Marzola, V., & Mascetti, G. G. (2000b). Visual-spatial attention in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 36(1), 109-123.
  167. Facoetti, A., Turatto, M., Lorusso, M. L., & Mascetti, G. G. (2001). Orienting of visual attention in dyslexia: Evidence for asymmetric hemispheric control of attention. Experimental Brain Research, 138, 46-53.
  168. Facoetti, A., Zorzi, M. Cestnick, L., Lorusso, M. L., Molteni, M., Paganoni, P., Umiltà, C., & Mascetti, G. G. (2006). The relationship between visuo-spatial attention and nonword reading in developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(6), 841-855.
  169. Fawcett, A. J., & Nicolson, R. I. (1999). Performance of dyslexic children on cerebellar and cognitive tests. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 31, 68-78.
  170. Fawcett, A., Nicolson, R., & Dean, P. (1996). Impaired performance of children with dyslexia on a range of cerebellar tasks. Annals of Dyslexia, 46, 259-283.
  171. Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2003). Early word learners’ ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words. Language and Speech, 46, 245-264.
  172. Ferrand, L. (1995). Evaluation du rôle de l’information phonologique dans l’identification des mots écrits. L’Année Psychologique, 95, 293-315.
  173. Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1992). Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition: Evidence from masked non-word priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45(3), 353-372.
  174. Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1993). The time-course of orthographic and phonological code activation in the early phases of visual word recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 119-122.
  175. Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1994). Effects of orthography are independent of phonology in masked form priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(2), 365-382.
  176. Ferrand, L., & Grainger, J. (1995). Amorçage phonologique masqué et dénomination. L’Année Psychologique, 95, 645-659.
  177. Ferrand, L., & New, B. (2003). Syllabic length effects in visual word recognition and naming. Acta Psychologica, 113, 167-183.
  178. Ferrand, L., Segui, J., & Grainger, J. (1996). Masked priming of word and picture naming: The role of syllabic units. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 708-723.
  179. Fiez, J. A., & Peterson, S. E. (1998). Neuroimaging studies of word reading. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95, 914-921.
  180. Fikkert, P. (2007). Acquiring phonology. In P. de Lacy (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology (pp. 537-554). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  181. Finch, A., Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. (2002). Evidence for a neuroanatomical difference within the olivo-cerebellar pathway of adults with dyslexia. Cortex, 38, 529- 539.
  182. Flege, J. E., & Eefting, W. (1986). Linguistic and developmental effects on the production and perception of stop consonants. Phonetica, 43, 155-171.
  183. Forrest, K., & Morrisette, M. L. (1999). Feature analysis of segmental errors in children with phonological disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 187-194.
  184. Fowler, C. A. (1987). Consonant-vowel cohesiveness in speech production as revealed by initial and final consonant exchanges. Speech Communication, 6, 231-244.
  185. Fowler, C., Treiman, R., & Gross, J. (1993). The structure of English syllables and polysyllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 115-140.
  186. Friedmann, N., & Nachman-Katz, I. (2004). Developmental neglect dyslexia in a Hebrew reading child. Cortex, 40, 301-313.
  187. Fromkin, V. A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 47, 27-52.
  188. Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71-99.
  189. Frost, R., & Kampf, M. (1993). Phonetic recoding of phonologically ambiguous printed words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 23-33.
  190. Frost, R., & Yogev, O. (2001). Orthographic and phonological computation in visual word recognition: Evidence from backward masking in Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(3), 525-530.
  191. Funnell, E. (1983). Phonological processes in reading. New evidence from acquired dyslexia. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 159-180.
  192. Galaburda, A. M., & Kemper, T. L. (1979). Cytoarchitectonic abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: A case study. Annals of Neurology, 6, 94-100.
  193. Galaburda, A. M., Menard, M. T., & Rosen, G. D. (1994). Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the USA, 16, 8010-8013.
  194. Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., Rosen, G. D., Aboitiz, F., & Geschwind, N. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: Four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18, 222–233.
  195. Garnes, S., & Bond, Z. S. (1975). Slips of the ear: Errors in perception of casual speech. Proceedings of the 11th Reg. Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 214-225.
  196. Garnica, O. K. (1973). The development of phonemic speech perception. In T. E. Moore (Eds.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 215-222). New York: Academic Press.
  197. Gathercole, S.E., Pickering, S.J., Hall, M., & Peaker, S.M. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological influences on serial recognition and serial recall. The Quarlerly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(1), 1-30.
  198. Geiger, G., Lettvin, J.Y., & Fahle, M. (1994). Dyslexic children learn a new visual strategy for reading: A controlled experiment. Vision Research, 34(9), 1223-1233.
  199. Génard, N., Mousty, P., Content, A., Alegria, J., Leybaert, J., & Morais, J. (1998). Methods to establish subtypes of developmental dyslexia. In P. Reitsma & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Problems and interventions in literacy development (pp.163-176). The Netherlands: Kluwer, Dordrecht.
  200. Georgiewa, P., Rzanny, R., Hopf, J. M., Knab, R., Glauche, V., & coll. (1999). fMRI during word processing in dyslexic and normal reading children. Neuroreport, 10, 3459-3465.
  201. Georgiewa, P., Rzanny, R., Gaser, C., Gerhard, U. J., Vieweg, U., & coll. (2002). Phonological processing in dyslexic children: A study combining functional imaging and event related potentials. Neuroscience Letter, 318, 5-8.
  202. Gerken, L., Murphy, W. D., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). 3-year-olds’ and 4-year-olds’ perceptual confusions for spoken words. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 475-486.
  203. Geudens, A., & Sandra, D. (1999). Onsets and rimes in a phonologically transparent orthography: Differences between good and poor beginning readers of Dutch. Brain and Language, 68, 284-290.
  204. Giraud, K., Démonet, J. F., Habib, M., Marquis, P., Chauvel, P., & Liegeois-Chauvel, C. (2005). Auditory evoked potential patterns to voiced and voiceless speech sounds in adult developmental dyslexics with persistent deficits. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1524-1534.
  205. Gizewski, E. R., Timmann, D., & Forsting, M. (2004). Specific cerebellar activation during Braille reading in blind subjects. Human Brain Mapping, 22, 229-235.
  206. Godfrey, J. J., Syrdal-Lasky, A. K., Millay, K. K., & Knox, C. M. (1981). Performance of dyslexic children on speech perception tests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 401-424.
  207. Goldinger, S. D., Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1989). Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 501-518.
  208. Goldinger, S. D., Luce, P. A., Pisoni, D. B., & Marcario, J. K. (1992). Form-based priming in spoken word recognition: The roles of competition and bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18(6), 1211-1238.
  209. Goldstein, L. M., & Fowler, C. (2003). Articulatory phonology: A phonology for public language use. In A. S. Meyer & N. O. Schiller (Eds.), Phonetics and phonology in language comprehension and production: Differences and similarities (pp. 159-207). Berlin : Mouton de Gruyter.
  210. Goswami, U. (2003). Why theories about developmental dyslexia require developmental designs. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 534-540.
  211. Goulandris, N. K., & Snowling, M. (1991). Visual memory deficits: A plausible cause of developmental dyslexia? Evidence from a single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 127-154.
  212. Goswami, U. (2002). In the beginning was the rhyme? A reflection on Hulme, Hatcher, Nation, Brown, Adams and Stuart, 2001. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 47-57.
  213. Gow, D. W., & Caplan, D. (1996). An examination of impaired acoustic-phonetic processing in aphasia. Brain and Language, 52, 386-407.
  214. Graham, L. W., & House, A. S. (1970). Phonological oppositions in children: A perceptual study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 559-566.
  215. Grainger, J., Bouttevin, S., Truc, C., Bastien, M., & Ziegler, J. (2003). Word superiority, pseudoword superiority, and learning to read: A comparison of dyslexic and normal readers. Brain and Language, 87, 432-440.
  216. Grainger, J., & Ferrand, L. (1996). Masked orthographic and phonological priming in visual word recognition and naming: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 623-647.
  217. Grant, A. C., Zangaladze, A., Thiagarajah, M. C., & Sathian, K. (1999). Tactile perception in developmental dyslexia: A psychophysical study using gratings. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1201-1211.
  218. Greenberg, J. H., & Jenkins, J. J. (1964). Studies in the psychological correlates of the sound system of American English. Word, 20, 157-177.
  219. Griffiths, S., & Frith, U. (2002). Evidence for an articulatory awareness deficit in adult dyslexics. Dyslexia, 8, 14-21.
  220. Gross, J., Treiman, R., & Inman, J. (2000). The role of phonology in a letter detection task. Memory and Cognition, 28(3), 349-357.
  221. Gupta, P., Lipinski, J., & Aktunc, E. (2005). Reexamining the phonological similarity effect in immediate serial recall: The roles of type of similarity, category cuing, and item recall. Memory and Cognition, 33(6), 1001-1016.
  222. Habib, M. (2002). Phonology, phonetics, and temporal processing in developmental dyslexia: From mechanisms to remediation. Paper presented at The ISCA Workshop on Temporal Integration in the Perception of Speech (TIPS), Aix-en-Provence: France.
  223. Habib, M. (2002). Aspects étiologiques des dyslexies. Paper presented at the XXXèmes Entretiens de Médecine Physique et de Réadaptation, Montpellier: France.
  224. Habib, M. (2001). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: An overview and working hypothesis. Brain, 123(12), 2373-2399.
  225. Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia. An overview and working hypothesis. Brain, 123, 2373-2399.
  226. Habib, M., Rey, V., Giraud, K., & Espesser, R. (1999). Dyslexie et traitement par le cerveau des sons de parole. Rencontres IPSEN en ORL, 3, 147-158.
  227. Habib, M., Robichon, F., & Démonet, J.-F. (1996). Le singulier cerveau des dyslexiques. La Recherche, 289, 80-85.
  228. Haggard, M. P. (1971). Encoding and the REA for speech signals. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23(1), 34-45.
  229. Hahn, U., & Bailey, T. M. (2005). What makes words sound similar? Cognition, 97, 227-267.
  230. Halle, M. (1992). “Features”. In B Wright (Ed.), Oxford International Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Oxford University Press: New-York.
  231. Hallé, P., & Boysson-Bardies, B. de (1996). The format of representation of recognized words in infants’ early receptive lexicon. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 463-481.
  232. Hallé, P., & Boysson-Bardies, B. de (1994). Emergence of an early lexicon: Infants’ recognition of words. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 119-129.
  233. Hanley, J. R., & Gard, F. (1995). A dissociation between developmental surface and phonological dyslexia in two undergraduate students. Neuropsychologia, 33, 909-914.
  234. Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 525-532.
  235. Hari, R., Renvall, H., & Tanskanen, T. (2001). Left minineglect in dyslexic adults. Brain, 124, 1373-1380.
  236. Hartline, H. K., & Ratcliff, F. (1957). Inhibitory interaction of receptor units in the eye of Limulus. Journal of General Physiology, 40, 357-376.
  237. Haspelmath, M. (2006). Against markedness (and what to replace it with). Journal of Linguistics, 42(1), 25-70.
  238. Hawelka, S., & Wimmer, H. (2005). Impaired visual processing of multi-element arrays is associated with increased number of eye movements in dyslexic reading. Vision Research, 45, 855-863.
  239. Hazan, V., & Barrett, S. (2000). The development of phonemic categorization in children aged 6-12. Journal of Phonetics, 28, 377-396.
  240. Hebben, N. (1986). The role of the frontal and temporal lobes in the phonetic organization of speech stimuli: A multidimensional scaling analysis. Brain and Language, 29, 342-357.
  241. Heilman, K. M., Voeller, K., & Alexander, A. W. (1996). Developmental dyslexia: A motorarticulatory feedback hypothesis. Annals of Neurology, 39, 407–412.
  242. Heim, S., & Keil, A. (2004). Large-scale neural correlates of developmental dyslexia. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 125–140.
  243. Helenius, P., Salmelin, R., Richardson, U., Leinonen, S., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(4), 603-617.
  244. Helenius, P., Tarkiainen, A., Cornelissen, P., Hansen, P. C., & Salmelin, R. (1999). Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 476-483.
  245. Hesling, I., Dilharreguy, B., Clément, S., Bordessoules, M., & Allard, M. (2005). Cerebral mechanisms of prosodic sensory integration using low-frequency bands of connected speech. Human Brain Mapping, 26, 157-169.
  246. Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Priming effects with phonemically similar words: The encoding-bias hypothesis reconsidered. Memory & Cognition, 8(2), 115-123.
  247. Hintzman, D. L. (1967). Articulatory coding in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 312-316.
  248. Houston, D. M., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2000). The role of talker-specific information in word segmentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1570-1582.
  249. Hugdahl, K., & Andersson, L. (1984). A dichotic listening study of differences in cerebral organization in dextral and sinistral subjects. Cortex, 20(1), 135-141.
  250. Hugdahl, K., & Wester, K. (1992). Dichotic listening studies of hemispheric asymmetry in brain damaged patients. International Journal of Neuroscience, 63(1-2), 17-29.
  251. Hutzler, F., Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. M (2005). Effects of syllable-frequency in lexical decision and naming: An eye-movement study. Brain and Language, 92, 138-152.
  252. Ivry, R. B., & Justus, T. C. (2001). A neural instantiation of the motor theory of speech perception. TINS, 24, 513-515.
  253. Jaeger, J. J. (1992). Phonetic features in young children’s slips of the tongue. Language and Speech, 35, 189-205.
  254. Jakobson, R., Fant, C. G. M., & Halle, M. (1952). Preliminaries to speech analysis: The distinctive features and their correlates. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institue of Technology Press.
  255. Jenner, A. R., Galaburda, A. M., & Sherman, G. F. (2000). Connectivity of ectopic neurons in the molecular layer of the somatosensory cortex in autoimmune mice. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 1005-1013.
  256. Jenner, A. R., Rosen, G. D., & Galaburda, A. M. (1999). Neuronal asymmetries primary visual cortex of dyslexic and nondyslexic brains. Annals of Neurology, 46(2), 189-196.
  257. Jimenez-Gonzalez, J. E., & Ramirez-Santana, G. (2002). Identifying subtypes of reading disability in a transparent orthography. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 5, 3-19.
  258. Jimenez-Gonzalez, J. E., & Valle, I. H. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 44-60.
  259. Joanisse, M. F., Manis, F. R., Keating, P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2000). Language deficits in dyslexic children: Speech perception, phonology, and morphology. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 30-60.
  260. Johannes, S., Kussmaul, C. L., Munte, T. F., & Mangun, G. R. (1996). Developmental dyslexia: Passive visual stimulation provides no evidence for a magnocellular processing defect. Neuropsychologia, 34, 1123-1127.
  261. Johnston, R. S., & Conning, A. (1990). The effects of overt and covert rehearsal on the emergence of the phonological similarity effect in 5-year-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8(4), 411-418.
  262. Johnston, R. B., Stark, R. E., Mellits, E. D., & Tallal, P. (1981). Neurological status of language-impaired and normal children. Annals of Neurology, 10, 159-163.
  263. Jorm, A. F., Share, D. L., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. C. (1986a). Cognitive factors at school entry predictive of specific reading retardation and general reading backwardness: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 5-54.
  264. Jorm, A. F., Share, D. L., Matthews, R. C., & Maclean, R. (1986b). Behaviour problems in specific reading retarded and general reading backward children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 33-43.
  265. Juphard, A., Carbonnel, S., Ans, B., & Valdois, S. (2006). Length effect in reading and lexical decision: The multitrace memory model’s account. Current Psychology Letters.
  266. Jusczyk, P. W. (1977). Perception of syllable-final stop consonants by 2-month-old infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 21(5), 450–454.
  267. Jusczyk, P. W., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 1-23.
  268. Jusczyk, P. W., & Derrah, C. (1987). Representation of speech sounds by young infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 648–654.
  269. Jusczyk, P. W., Goodman, M. B., & Baumann, A. (1999). Nine-month-olds’ attention to sound similarities in syllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 62-82.
  270. Jusczyk, P. W., Pisoni, D. B., Walley, A. C., & Murray, J. (1980). Discrimination of the relative onset time of two-component tones by infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 67, 262–270.
  271. Keïta, L. (2007). Approche développementale et neuropsychologique de processus visuo-attentionnels: traitements global et local selon la catégorie. Thèse de Doctorat de Psychologie, Université Lumière Lyon 2 (soutenue le 04 juin 2008).
  272. Keïta, L., Bedoin, N., Herbillon, V., Lévy-Sebbag, H., & Mérigot, A. (2005, novembre). Visual attention deficits and linguistic impairments in normal readers and dyslexic children.Communication affichée au XVIIth European Academy of Childhood Disability Congress. Monaco: Principauté de Monaco.
  273. Kéïta, L., Bedoin, N., Mérigot, A., & Herbillon, V. (2005, septembre). Global/local processing of letter-based and object-based hierarchical stimuli: A developmental and neuropsychological study. Communication affichée à XIVth Conference of the European Society of Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP 14). Leiden: Holland.
  274. Kibel, M., & Miles, T. R. (1994). Phonological errors in the spelling of taught dyslexic children. In C. Hulme & M. Snowling (Eds.), Reading development adn dyslexia (pp.105-127). London: Whurr.
  275. Kimura, D. (1967). Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex, 3, 163-168.
  276. Klatt, D. H. (1979). Speech perception: A model of acoustic-phonetic analysis and lexical access. Journal of Phonetics, 7, 279-312.
  277. Kluender, K. R., Lotto, A. J., & Jenison, R. L. (1995). Perception of voicing for syllable-initial stops at different intensities: Does synchrony capture signal voiceless stop consonant? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 2552-2567.
  278. Kraus, N., McGee, T. J., Carrell, T. D., Zecker, S.G., Nicol, T. G., & Koch, D. B. (1996). Auditory neurophysiologic responses and discrimination deficits in children with learning problems. Science, 273, 971-973.
  279. Krifi-Papoz, S. (2009, mars). Traitement phonétique en lecture et dyslexie développementale: anomalies de l’organisation fine des connaissances phonologiques chez les enfants dyslexiques. Communication orale au Colloque Neuropsychologie, Education et Francophonie (NEF), Ecole Normale Supérieure, Lyon: France.
  280. Krifi, S. (2003). Amorçage et masquage phonétique intra-stimulus en lecture: enfants normo-lecteurs et dyslexiques. Mémoire de DEA National de Neuropsychologie, Université Lumière Lyon 2.
  281. Krifi, S., & Bedoin, N. (2005, février). Rôle de trois catégories de traits phonétiques dans l’appariement de syllables écrites: lecture normale et dyslexie développementale. Poster présenté au Colloque Un siècle de Phonétique expérimentale: histoire et développement de Théodore Rosset à John Ohala, Grenoble: France.
  282. Krifi, S., & Bedoin, N. (2004, octobre). Traits phonétiques en lecture: influence de la présentation simultanée des lettres d’un stimulus et de leur temps de présentation. Conférence présentée au Workshop Structure and Dynamics of the Phonetic-Phonological Systems, Lyon: France.
  283. Krifi, S., Bedoin, N., & Herbillon, V. (2005a, septembre). The hierarchy of phonetic features categories in printed syllables matching: Normal reading and developmental dyslexia. Poster presented at the XIVth Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP 14), Leiden: Holland.
  284. Krifi, S., Bedoin, N., & Herbillon, V. (2005b, novembre). Hierarchy of phonetic categories in developmental dyslexia. Conference presented at the XVIIth European Academy of Chilhood Disability Congress, Monaco: Principauté de Monaco.
  285. Krifi, S., Bedoin, N., & Herbillon, V. (2005c, mars). Premiers pas vers un outil de remediation pour enfants dyslexiques: appariement de syllabes et lecture. Poster présenté au Colloque APEDYS, Lyon: France.
  286. Krifi, S., Bedoin, N., & Herbillon, V. (2004). Effets phonétiques en lecture: une meilleure compréhension de la lecture et de la dyslexie. Conférence à la Réunion de Neuropédiatrie et Neuropsychologie, Centre Hospitalier Lyon-Sud, Pierre-Bénite: France.
  287. Krifi, S., Bedoin, N., & Herbillon, V. (2003, septembre). Phonetic priming and backward masking in printed stimuli: A better understandig of normal reading and dyslexia. Paper presented at the XIIIth Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP), Granada: Spain.
  288. Krifi-Papoz, S., Bedoin, N., Herbillon, V., des Portes, V., & Rousselle, C. (2007, octobre). Modèle d’organisation des connaissances sur les phonèmes: une explication des erreurs de lecture et d’écriture des enfants dyslexiques. Communication affichée aux IIIèmes Journées Scientifiques du Centre de Référence des Troubles des Apprentissages de Lyon, Ecole Normale Supérieure, Lyon: France.
  289. Krifi, S., Bedoin, N., & Mérigot, A. (2003). Effects of voicing similarity between consonants in printed stimuli, in normal and dyslexic readers. Current Psychology Letters: Behavior, Brain & Cognition, 10(1), 1-7.
  290. Krifi, S., Mérigot, A., & Bedoin, N. (2002, novembre). Traitement de la ressemblance de voisement en lecture et dyslexie. Poster présenté aux IIIe Journées des Troubles d’Apprentissage du Langage Oral et Ecrit, Aix-en-Provence: France.
  291. Kronbichler, M., Hutzler, F., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Dyslexia: Verbal impairments in the absence of magnocellular impairments. Neuroreport, 13, 617-620.
  292. Kuhl, P. K. (1983). Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 263-285.
  293. Kuhl, P. K., & Miller, J. D. (1975). Speech perception by the chinchilla: Voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants. Science, 190, 69–72.
  294. Kuhl, P. K., & Padden, D. M. (1983). Enhanced discriminability at the phonetic boundaries for the place feature in macaques. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 73, 1003–1010.
  295. Kuhl, P. K., & Padden, D. M. (1982). Enhanced discriminability at the phonetic boundaries for the voicing feature in macaques. Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 542–550.
  296. Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9(2), 13-21.
  297. Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experiences alter phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255, 606-608.
  298. Kujala, T., & Naatanen, R. (2001). The mismatch negativity in evaluating central auditory dysfunction in dyslexia. Neuroscience and Biobehaviour Review, 25, 535-543.
  299. Laberge, D., & Brown, V. (1989). Theory of attentional operation in shape identification. Psychological Review, 96(1), 101-124.
  300. Laboratoire Cogni-Sciences et Apprentissages, & IUFM Grenoble (1999). Diagnostic Analytique des Troubles d’Apprentissage de la Lecture.
  301. Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthography consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315-334.
  302. Lefavrais, P. (1965). Test de l’Alouette. Paris: E.C.P.A.
  303. Leonard, C., Eckert, M., Lombardino, L., Oakland, T., Kranzler, J., & coll. (2001). Anatomical risk factors for phonological dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 148-157.
  304. Leonard, C. M., Voeller, K. K. S., Lombardino, L. J., Morris, M. K., Hynd, G. W., Alexander, A. W., Andersen, H. G., & al. (1993). Anomalous cerebral structure in dyslexia revealed with magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of neurology, 50, 461-469.
  305. Lesch, M. F., & Pollatsek, A. (1993). Automatic access of semantic information by phonological codes in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 285-294.
  306. Lesch, M. F., & Pollatsek, A. (1998). Evidence for the use of assembled phonology in accessing the meaning of printed words. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(3), 573-592.
  307. Lian, A. L., & Karlsen, P . J. (2004). Advantage and disadvantages of phonological similarity in serial recall and serial recognition of nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 32(2), 223-234.
  308. Liberman, A. M., Delattre, P. C., & Cooper, F. S. (1958). Some cues for the distinction between voiced and voiceless stops in initial position. Language and Speech, 1, 153-167.
  309. Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., & Griffith, B. C. (1957). The discrimination of speech sounds within and across phoneme boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 358-368.
  310. Liberman, I. Y., Mann, V. A., Shankweiler, D., & Werfelman, M. (1982). Children’s memory for recurring linguistic and non-linguistic material in relation to reading ability, Cortex, 18, 367-375.
  311. Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fisher, F. W., & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201-212.
  312. Lindgren, S. D., De Renzi, E., & Richman, L. C. (1985). Cross-national comparisons of developmental dyslexia in Italy and the United States. Child Development, 56, 1404-1417.
  313. Lisker, L., & Abranson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20, 384-422.
  314. Livingstone, M. S., Rosen, G. D., Drislane, F. W., & Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in develomental dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences-USA, 88, 7643-7647.
  315. Logan, J. (1992). Computation of young children’s lexicons. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN.
  316. Lorenzi, C., Dumont, A., & Fullgrabe, C. (2000). Use of temporal envelope cues by children with developmental dyslexia. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 43, 1367-1379.
  317. Lovegrove, W., Martin, F., & Slaghuis, W. (1986). A theoretical and experimental case for a visual deficit in reading disability. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 225-267.
  318. Luce, P. A. (1986). Neighborhoods of words in the mental lexicon. PhD Dissertation, Dept. of Psychology, Indiana University, Bloomington Indiana.
  319. Luce, P. A., Goldinger, S. D., Auer, E. T., & Vitevitch, M. S. (2000). Phonetic priming, neighborhood activation, and PARSYN. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(3), 615-625.
  320. Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear & Hearing, 19, 1-36.
  321. Luce, P. A., Pisoni, D. B., & Goldinger, S. D. (1990). Similarity neighborhoods of spoken words. In G.T.M. Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing (pp. 122-147). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  322. Lukatela, G., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1990). Phonemic priming with words and pseudowords. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 2, 375-394.
  323. Lukatela, G., Eaton, T., Lee, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2001). Does visual word identification involve a sub-phonemic level? Cognition, 78, 41-52.
  324. Lukatela, G., Eaton, T., Sabadini, L., & Turvey, M. T. (2004). Vowel duration affects visual word identification: Evidence that the mediating phonology is phonetically informed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human, Perception, and Performance, 30(1), 151-162.
  325. Lukatela, G., Frost, R., & Turvey, M. T. (1998). Phonological priming by masked nonword primes in the lexical decision task. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 666-683.
  326. Lukatela, G., Lukatela, K., & Turvey, M. T. (1993). Further evidence for phonological constraints on visual lexical access: TOWED primes FROG. Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 461-466.
  327. Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M.T. (1994). Visual lexical access is initially phonological: Evidence from phonological priming by homophones and pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 331-353.
  328. Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1993). Similar attentional, frequency, and associative effects for pseudohomophones and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human, Perception and Performance, 19, 166-178.
  329. Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M. T. (1991). Phonological access of the lexicon: Evidence from associative priming with pseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 951-966.
  330. Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O. P. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263-284.
  331. Luo, C. R., Johnson, R. A., & Gallo, D. A. (1998). Automatic activation of phonological information in reading: Evidence from the semantic relatedness decision task. Memory & Cognition, 26(4), 833-843.
  332. Maassen, B., Groenen, P., Crul, T., Assman-Hulsmans, C., & Gabreëls, F. (2001). Identification and discrimination of voicing and place-of-articulation in developmental dyslexia. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 15(4), 319-339.
  333. MacKay, D. G. (1970). Spoonerisms: The structure of errors in the serial order of speech. Neuropsychologia, 8, 323-350.
  334. Mani, N., & Plunkett, K. (2007). Fourteen-month-olds pay attention to vowels in novel words. Developmental Science, 11(1), 53-59.
  335. Manis, F., McBride-Chang, C., Seidenberg, M. S., Keating, P., Doi, L. M., Munson, B., & Petersen, A. (1997). Are speech perception deficits associated with developmental dyslexia? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 211-235.
  336. Manis, F., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Peterson, A. (1996). On the basis of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157-195.
  337. Mann, V. A., & Liberman, I. Y. (1984). Phonological awareness and verbal short term memory: Can they presage early reading problems? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 592-599.
  338. Marendaz, C., Valdois, S., & Walch, J. P. (1996). Dyslexie développementale et attention visuo-spatiale. L’Année Psychologique, 96, 193-224.
  339. Marshall, C. M., Snowling, M. J., & Bailey, P. J. (2001). Rapid auditory processing and phonological ability in normal readers and readers with dyslexia. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 44, 925-940.
  340. Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition, 25, 71-102.
  341. Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Moss, H. E., & Van Halen, S. (1996). Perceptual distance and competition in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1376-1392.
  342. Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Warren, P. (1994). Levels of perceptual representation and process on lexical access: Words, phonemes, and features. Psychological Review, 101, 653-675.
  343. Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Welsh, A. (1978). Processing interactions and lexical access during word recognition in continuous speech. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 29-63.
  344. Masterson, J., Hazan, V., & Wijayatilake, L. (1995). Phonemic processing problems in developmental phonological dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12(3), 233-259.
  345. Mathey, S., Zagar, D. (2002). Lexical similarity in visual word recognition: The effect of syllabic neighbourhood in French. Current Psychology Letters: Behavior, Brain, & Cognition, 8, 107-121.
  346. Mathey, S., Zagar, D., Doignon, N., & Seigneuric, A. (2006). The nature of the syllabic neighbourhood effect in French. Acta Psychologica, 123, 372-393.
  347. Maye, J., & Gerken, L. A. (2001). Learning phonemes: How far can the input take us? Proceedings of the BUCLD 25 (pp. 480-490). Cascadilla Press: Somerville, MA.
  348. Maye, J., Weiss, D. J., & Aslin, R. N. (2008). Statistical phonetic learning in infants: Facilitation and feature generalization. Developmental Science, 11(1), 122-134.
  349. McAnally, K. I., & Stein, J. F. (1996). Auditory temporal coding in dyslexia. Proceedings Royal Society London, B, Biological Sciences, 263, 961-965.
  350. McCarthy, J. J. (1988). Feature geometry and dependency: A review. Phonetica, 43, 84-108.
  351. McClaskey, C., Pisoni, D., & Carrell, T.D. (1983). Transfer of training of a new linguistic contrast in voicing. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 323-330.
  352. McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1-86.
  353. McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375-407.
  354. McCormack, P., & Dodd, B. (1996). A feature analysis of speech errors in subgroups of speech disordered children. In P. McCormack, & A. Russell (Eds.), Proceedings of the Sixth Australian International Conference on Speech Science and Technology (pp. 217-222). Adelaide: AASTA.
  355. McDougall, S., Hulme, C., Ellis, A., & Monk, A. (1994). Learning to read: The role of short term memory and phonological skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 112-133.
  356. McInish, J. R., & Tikofsky, R. S. (1969). Distinctive features and response latencies: A pilot study. Perception & Psychophysics, 6, 267-268.
  357. McKibbin, K., Elias, L. J., Saucier, D. M., & Engebregston, D. (2003). Right-hemispheric dominance for processing extended non-linguistic frequency transitions. Brain and Cognition, 53, 322-326.
  358. McMurray, B., & Aslin, R. (2005). Infants are sensitive to within-category variation in speech perception. Cognition, 95(2), B15-26.
  359. McMurray, B., Tanenhaus, M., & Aslin, R. (2002). Gradient effects of within-category phonetic variation on lexical access. Cognition, 86(2), 33-42.
  360. McQueen, J. M., Dahan, D., & Cutler, A. (2003). Continuity and gradedness in speech processing. In N. O. Schiller & A. S. Meyer (Eds.), Phonetics and phonology in language comprehension and production: Differences and similarities (pp. 39-79). Berlin: Mouton & de Gruyter.
  361. Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 3-19.
  362. Metsala, J. L., Stanovich, K. E., & Brown, G. D. A. (1998). Regularity effects and the phonological deficit model of reading disabilities: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 279-293.
  363. Meyer, A. (1992). Investigation of phonological encoding through speech error analyses: Achievements, limitations, and alternatives. Cognition, 42, 181-211.
  364. Meyer , D. E., & Gordon, P. C. (1985). Speech production: Motor programming of phonetic features. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 3-26.
  365. Meyer, D. E., & Gordon, P. C. (1983). Shared mechanisms for perceiving and producing phonetic features in speech. Proceedings of the Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, San Diego, CA.
  366. Miceli, G., Caltagirone, C., Gainotti, G., & Payer-Rigo, P. (1978). Discrimination of voice versus place contrasts in aphasia. Brain and Language, 6, 47-51.
  367. Milberg, W., Blumstein, S., & Dworetzky, B. (1988). Phonological factors in lexical access: Evidence from an auditory lexical decision task. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 26(4), 305-308.
  368. Miller, J. L. (1997). Internal structure of phonetic categories. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 865-869.
  369. Miller, J. L., & Eimas, P. (1995). Speech perception: From signal to word. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 467-492.
  370. Miller, J. L., & Eimas, P. (1983). Studies on the categorization of speech by infants. Cognition, 13, 135-165.
  371. Miller, G. A., & Nicely, P. E. (1955). An analysis of perceptual confusions among some english consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27(2), 338-352.
  372. Mills, S., Coffey, S., & Neville, H. (1993). Language acquisition and cerebral specialization in 20-month-old infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 317-334.
  373. Milner, B., Taylor, L., & Sperry, R. W. (1968). Lateralized suppression of dichotically presented digits after commissural section in man. Science, 161(837), 184-186.
  374. Mody, M. (1993). Bases of reading impairment in speech perception: A deficit in rate of auditory processing or in phonological coding? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Speech and Hearing Sciences, City University of New York.
  375. Mody, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Brady, S. (1997). Speech perception deficits in poor readers: Auditory processing or phonological coding? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 199-231.
  376. Molfese, D. L. (1978). Neural correlates of categorical speech perception in adults. Brain and Language, 5, 25-35.
  377. Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1988). Right-hemisphere responses from preschool children to temporal cues to speech and non-speech materials: Electrophysiological correlates. Brain and Language, 33, 245-259.
  378. Montgomery, D. (1981). Do dyslexics have difficulty accessing articulatory information. Psychological Research, 43, 235-243.
  379. Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (1994). Perception and awareness in phonological processing: the case of the phoneme. Cognition, 50, 287-297.
  380. Mueller, S. T., Seymour, T. L., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E. (2003). Theoretical implications of articulatory duration, and phonological complexity in verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1353-1380.
  381. Myers, E. B., Blumstein, S. E., Walsh, E., & Eliassen, J. (2009). Inferior frontal regions underlie the perception of phonetic category invariance. Psychological Science, 20(7), 895-903.
  382. Nagarajan, S., Mahncke, H., Salz, T., Tallal, P., Roberts, T., & Merzenich, M.M. (1999). Cortical auditory signal processing in poor readers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences-USA, 96, 6483-6488.
  383. Naish, P. (1980). The effects of graphemic and phonemic similarity between targets and masks in a backward visual masking paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 57-68.
  384. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353-383.
  385. Newman, R. L. & Connolly, J. F. (2004). Determining the role of phonology in silent reading using event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 94-105.
  386. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1994). Reaction times and dyslexia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,, 47, 29-48.
  387. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1990). Automaticity: A new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition, 35, 159-182.
  388. Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A.J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: The cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(9), 508-511.
  389. Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (1995). Time estimation deficits in developmental dyslexia: Evidence for cerebellar involvement. Proceedings of the Royal Society, 259, 43–47.
  390. Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., Berry, E. L., Jenkins, I. H., Dean, P., & Brooks, D. J. (1999). Association of abnormal cerebellar activation with motor learning difficulties in dyslexic adults. Lancet, 353, 1662-1167.
  391. Nittrouer, S. (1999). Do temporal processing deficits cause phonological processing problems? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(4), 925-942.
  392. Nittrouer, S., & Miller, M. E. (1997). Predicting developmental shifts in perceptual weighting schemes. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 2253-2266.
  393. Norris, D. (1994). Shortlist: A connextionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52, 189-234.
  394. Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113.
  395. Ortiz, R., Jimenez, J. E., & Miranda, E. G. (2007). Locus and nature of perceptual phonological deficit in spanish children with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(1), 80-92.
  396. Oscar-Berman, M., Zurif, E. B., & Blumstein, S. (1975). Effects of unilateral brain damage on the processing of speech sounds. Brain and Language, 2, 345-355.
  397. Page, M. P. A., Madge, A., Cumming, N., & Norris, D. G. (2007). Speech errors and the phonological similarity effet in short-term memory: Evidence suggesting a common locus. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 49-64.
  398. Paulesu, E., Démonet, J. F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cossu, G., Habib, M., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2001). Dyslexia, Cultural diversity and Biological unity. Science, 291, 2165-2167.
  399. Paulesu, E., Frith, U., Snowling, M., Gallagher, A., Morton, J., Frackowiak, R.S.J., & Frith, C.D. (1996). Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from PET scanning. Brain, 119, 143-157.
  400. Pavlidis, G. T. (1981). Do eye movements hold the key to dyslexia? Neuropsychologia, 19, 57-64.
  401. Peereman, R. (1991). La médiation phonologique dans la reconnaissance des mots écrits. In R. Kolinsky, J. Morais, & J. Segui. (Eds.), La Reconnaissance des mots dans différentes modalités sensorielles (pp.119-163). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
  402. Pegg, J., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Adult and infant perception of two English phones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(6), 4742-4753.
  403. Pelli, D. G, Burns, C. W., Farrel, B., & Moore-Page, D. C. (2006). Feature detection and letter identification. Vision Research, 46, 4646-4674.
  404. Peng, D-L. Ding, G-S, Perry, C., Xu, D., Jin, Z., Luo, Q., Zhang, L., & Deng, Y. (2004). fMRI evidence for the automatic phonological activation of briefly presented words. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 156-164.
  405. Perecman, E., & Kellar, L. (1981). The effect of voice and place among aphasic, nonaphasic right-damaged, and normal subjects on a metalinguistic task. Brain and Language, 12, 213-223.
  406. Perfetti, C. A., & Bell, L. (1991). Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms of word identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 473-485.
  407. Perfetti, C. A., Bell, L. C., & Delaney, S. M. (1988). Automatic (prelexical) phonetic activation in silent word reading: Evidence from backward masking. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 59-70.
  408. Perfetti, C. A., & Zhang, S. (1991). Phonemic processes in reading chinese words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Language, 1, 633-643.
  409. Peter, M., & Turvey, M. T. (1994). Phonological codes are early sources of constraint in visual semantic categorization. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(5), 497-504.
  410. Peters, R.W. (1963). Dimension of perception of consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 35, 1985-1989.
  411. Pisoni, D. B., & Tash, J. (1974). Reaction times to comparisons with anda cross phonetic categories. Perception & Psychophysics, 15, 285-290.
  412. Poeppel, D. (2003). The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: Cerebral lateralization as ‘asymmetric sampling in time’. Speech Communication, 41, 245-255.
  413. Poeppel, D., Idsardi, W. J., & van Wassenhove, V. (2008). Speech perception at the interface of neurobiology and linguistics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 1071-1086.
  414. Polka, L., Colantonio, C., & Sundara, M. (2001). A cross-language comparison of /d/ - /D/ discrimination: Evidence for a new developmental pattern? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 2190-2201.
  415. Polka, L., & Werker, J. F. (1994). Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(2), 421-435.
  416. Post, Y. V., Foorman, B. R., Hiscock, M. (1997). Speech perception and speech production as indicators of reading difficulty. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 1-25.
  417. Prasada, S., & Pinker, S. (1993). Generalization of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 1-56.
  418. Price, C. J. (1998). The functional anatomy of word comprehension and production. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 281-288.
  419. Price, C. J., Warburton, E. A., Moore, C. J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Friston, K. J. (2001). Dynamic diaschisis: Anatomically remote and context-sensitive human brain lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 419-429.
  420. Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Jenner, A. R., Katz, L., Frost, S. J., Lee, J. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2000). Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia). Mental Retardation and Developemental Disability Research Review, 6, 207-213.
  421. Rack, J., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 29-53.
  422. Rack, J., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Wightman, J. (1994). The role of phonology in young children learning to read words: The direct-mapping hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 42-71.
  423. Rae, C., Harasty, J. A., Dzendrowskyj, T. E., Talcott, J. B., Simpson, J. M., Blamire, A., Dixon, R., Lee, M. A., Thompson, C., Styles, P., Richardson, A. J., & Stein, J. F. (2002). Cerebellar morphology in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 1357, 1–8.
  424. Rae, C., Lee, M. A., Dixon, R. M., Blamire, A. M., Thompson, C. H., Styles, P., Talcott, J., Richardson, A. J., & Stein, J. F. (1998). Metabolic abnormalities in developmental dyslexia detected by 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Lancet, 351, 1849-1852.
  425. Ramus, F., Pidgeon, E., & Frith, U. (2003a). The relationship between motor control and phonology in dyslexics children. Journal of Child Psycholgy and Psychiatry, 44, 712-722.
  426. Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U. (2003b). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841-865.
  427. Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Lesch, M. F., & Pollatsek, A. (1995). Phonological codes are automatically activated during reading: Evidence from an eye movement priming paradigm. Psychological Science, 6, 26-32.
  428. Rebattel, M., & Bedoin, N. (2001). Cerebral hemispheric asymmetry in voicing and manner of articulation processing in reading. Poster presented at the XIIth Conference of the European Society of Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP), Edinburg: Scotland.
  429. Reed, M. A. (1989). Speech perception and the discrimination of brief auditory cues in dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 270-292.
  430. Reitsma, P., & Vinke, J. (1986). Individuele verschillen in direct en indirect lezen. In P. Reitsme, A. G. Bus, & W. H. J. Van Bon (Eds.), Lerel lezen en spellen: Outwikkeling en problemen (pp. 80-85). The Netherlands, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
  431. Rey, A., Jacobs, A. M., Schmidt-Weigand, F., & Ziegler, J. C. (1998). A phoneme effect in visual word recognition. Cognition, 68, 71-80.
  432. Rey, V., de Martino, S., Espesser, R., & Habib, M. (2002). Temporal processing and phonological impairment in dyslexia: Effect of phoneme lengthening on order judgement of two consonants. Brain and Language, 80, 576-591.
  433. Rimol, L. M., Eichele, T., & Hugdahl, K. (2006). The effect of voice-onset-time on dichotic listening with consonant-vowel syllables. Neuropsychologia, 44, 191-196.
  434. Rogers, M.A., & Storkel, H.L. (1998). Reprogramming phonologically similar utterances: The role of phonetic features in pre-motor encoding. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 258-274.
  435. Rosen, S. (2003). Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: Is there a deficit? What is its nature? Does it explain anything? Journal of Phonetics, 31(3), 509-527.
  436. Rosen, S., & Manganari, E. (2001). Is there a relationship between speech and nonspeech auditory processing in children with dyslexia ? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 720-736.
  437. Rosenthal, R., Rosnow, R. L., & Rubin, D. B. (2000). Contrasts and effect sizes in behavioral research: A correlational approach. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  438. Rowse, H. J, & Wilshire, C. E. (2007). Comparison of phonological and whole-word treatments for two contrasting cases of developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(8), 817-842.
  439. Ruff, S., Cardebat, D., Marie, N., & Démonet, J. F. (2002). Enhanced response of the left frontal cortex to slowed down speech in dyslexia: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 13, 1285-1289.
  440. Ruff, S., Marie, N., Celsis, P., Cardebat, D., & Démonet, J. F. (2003). Neural substrates of impaired categorical perception of phonemes in adult dyslexics: An fMRI study. Brain and Cognition, 53, 331-334.
  441. Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In D. E. Rumelhart & J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 216-271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  442. Rumsey, J. M., Andreason, P., Zametkin, A. J., Aquino, T., King, A. C., Hamburger, S. D., Pikus, A., Rapoport, J. L., & Cohen, R. M. (1992). Failure to activate the left temporoparietal cortex in dyslexia: An oxygen 15 positron emission tomographic study. Archives of Neurology, 49, 527-534.
  443. Rumsey, J. M., Donohue, B. C., Brady, D. R., Nace, K., Giedd, J. N., & Andreason, P. (1997). A magnetic resonance imaging study of planum temporale asymmetry in men with developmental dyslexia. Archives of Neurology, 54, 1481-1489.
  444. Rumsey, J. M., Horwitz, B., Donohue, B. C., Nace, K. L., Maisog, J. M., & Andreason, P. A. (1999). Functional lesion in developmental dyslexia: Left angular gyral blood flow predicts severity. Brain and Language, 70, 187-204.
  445. Sagey, E. (1986). The representation of features and relations in nonlinear phonology. Doctoral Dissertation, MIT.
  446. Sanders, L. D., & Poeppel, D. (2007). Local and global auditory processing: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1172-1186.
  447. Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities. Phonological awareness and some other promising, predictors. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accrado, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75-119). New York: York Press.
  448. Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J., & Remschmidt, H. (1999). The role of phonological awareness, speech perception, and auditory temporal processing for dyslexia. European Child ans Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, 28-34.
  449. Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J., & Remschmidt, H. (1998). Auditory processing and dyslexia: Evidence for a specific speech processing deficit. Neuroreport, 9, 337-340.
  450. Schwippert, C. E., & Koopmans-Beinum, F. J. (1998). Phoneme boundary perception in dyslexic and normal-reading adults. Proceedings of the Institute of Phonetic Sciences Amsterdam, 22, 125–134.
  451. Segalowitz, N. S., & Cohen, H. (1989). Right hemisphere EEG sensitivity to speech. Brain and Language, 37, 220-231.
  452. Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Barnes, M. A., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition? Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 383-404.
  453. Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., Carré, R., & Démonet, J. F. (2001). Perceptual discrimination of speech sounds in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 384-399.
  454. Serniclaes, W., Van Heghe, S., Mousty, P., Carré, R., Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2004). Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 336-361.
  455. Serniclaes, W., Ventura, P., Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (2005). Categorical perception of speech sounds in illiterate adults. Cognition, 98, B35-B44.
  456. Sevald, C. A., & Dell, G. S. (1994). The sequential cuing effect in speech production. Cognition, 53, 91-127.
  457. Seymour, P. H. K. (1986). A cognitive analysis of dyslexia. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
  458. Seymour, P. H. K., & Duncan, L. G. (1997). Small versus large unit theories of reading acquisition. Dyslexia, 3, 125-134.
  459. Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151-218.
  460. Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (2002). Temporal processing and reading disability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 151-178.
  461. Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., & Klatt, D. H. (1979). The limited use of distinctive features and markedness in speech production: Evidence from speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 41-55.
  462. Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., Constable, R. T., Marchione, K. E., Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., & Gore, J. C. (2002). Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 101-110.
  463. Shelton, J. R., & Weinrich, M. (1997). Further evidence of a dissociation between output phonological and orthographic lexicons: A case study. In A. Caramazza (Ed.), Access of phonological and orthographic lexical forms: Evidence from dissociations in reading and spelling (pp. 105-131). UK: Psychology Press.
  464. Shepard, R. N. (1972). Psychological representation of speech sounds. In E. E. David & P. B. Denes (Eds.), Human communication: A unified view. New-York: McGraw-Hill.
  465. Shvachkin, N. K. (1966). Development of phonemic speech perception in early childhood. In F. Smith & G. A. Mille (Eds.), The genesis of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  466. Simon, C., & Fourcin, A. (1978). Cross-language study of speech-pattern learning. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63, 925-935.
  467. Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Bergman, E., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2000a). Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: A magnetic source imaging approach. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 809-816.
  468. Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J.M., Foorman, B. R., Bergman, E., Fishbeck, E., & Papanicolaou, A. C., (2000b). Brain activation in dyslexic in dyslexic children during non-word reading: A magnetic source imaging study. Cerebral Neuroscience Letters, 290, 61-65.
  469. Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Wheless, J. W., Maggio, W. W., Fletcher, J. M., Catillo, E. M., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2000c). Brain mechanisms for reading: The role of the superior temporal gyrus in word and pseudoword naming. Neuroreport, 11, 2443-2467.
  470. Simos, P. G., Molfese, D. L., & Brenden, R. A. (1997). Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of voicing-cue discrimination: Laterality patterns and development. Brain and Language, 57, 122-150.
  471. Singh, S., & Black, J. W. (1966). Study of twenty-six intervocalic consonants as spoken and recognized by four language groups. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 39(2), 372-387.
  472. Singh, L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2004). Preference and processing: The role of speech affect in early spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 173-189.
  473. Singh, S., Woods, D. R., & Becker, G. M. (1972). Perceptual structure of 22 prevocalic English consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 52, 1698-1713.
  474. Skottun, B.C. (2000). The magnocellular deficit theory of dyslexia: The evidence from contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 40, 111-127.
  475. Slaghuis, W. L., Lovegrove, W. J., & Davidson, J. (1993). Visual and language processing deficits are concurrent in dyslexia. Cortex, 29, 601-615.
  476. Slaghuis, W. L., & Ryan, J. F. (1999). Spatio-temporal contrast sensitivity, coherent motion, and visible persistence in developmental dyslexia. Vision Research, 39, 651-668.
  477. Slowiaczek, L. M., Nusbaum, H. C., & Pisoni, D. B. (1987). Phonological priming in auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 64-75.
  478. Snoeren, N. A., Hallé, P. A., & Segui, J. (2006). A voice for the voiceless: Production and perception of assimilated stops in French. Journal of Phonetics, 34, 241-268.
  479. Snow, K. (1964). A comparative study of sound substitutions by « normal » first grade children. Speech Monographs, 31, 135-141.
  480. Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford: Blackwell.
  481. Snowling, M. J., Stackhouse, J., & Rack, J. (1986). Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia: A developmental analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 309-339.
  482. Sommers, M. S., Kirk, K. I., & Pisoni, D. B. (1997). Some considerations in evaluating spoken word recognition by normal-hearing, noise-masked normal-hearing, and cochlear implant listeners. I: The effects of response format. Ear and Hearing, 18, 89-99.
  483. Sparks, R., & Geschwind, N. (1968). Dichotic listening in man after section of neocortical commissure, Cortex, 4, 3-16.
  484. Sperling, A. J., Lu, Z. L., Manis, F. R., & Seidenberg, M. (2003). Selective magnocellular deficits in dyslexia: A “phantom contour” study. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1422-1429.
  485. Spinelli, D., Angelelli, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Judica, A., & Zoccolotti, P. (1997). Developmental surface dyslexia is not associated with deficits in the transient visual system. Neuroreport, 8, 1807-1812.
  486. Spoehr, K. T. (1978). Phonological encoding in visual word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 127-141.
  487. Spoehr, K. T., & Smith, E. E. (1975). The role of orthographic and phonotactic rules in perceiving letter patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 104(1), 21-34.
  488. Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2003). Linguistic processes in reading and spelling. The case of alphabetic writing systems: English, French, German and Spanish. In T. Nunes, & P. Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children's literacy (pp. 43-65). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  489. Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2003). Lecture et Dyslexie. Paris: Dunod.
  490. Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Lacert, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2000). On subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from processing time and accuracy scores. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 87-104.
  491. Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Serniclaes, W. (2004). Nature et origine des déficits dans la dyslexie développementale: l’hypothèse phonologique. In S. Valdois, P. Colé, & D. David (Eds.), Apprentissage de la lecture et dyslexie (pp. 113-146). Marseille: Solal.
  492. Sroka, J. J., & Braida, L. D. (2005). Human and machine consonant recognition. Speech Communication, 45, 401-423.
  493. Stackhouse, J. (1992). Developmental verbal dyspraxia 1: A review and critique. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 27, 19-34.
  494. Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word learning tasks. Nature, 388, 381-382.
  495. Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 114-127.
  496. Stein, J. (2003). Visual motion sensitivity and reading. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1785-1793.
  497. Stein, J., & Talcott, J. (1999). Impaired neuronal timing in developmental dyslexia: The magnocellular hypothesis. Dyslexia, 5, 59-77.
  498. Stein, J., Talcott, J., & Walsh, V. (2000a). Controversy about the visual magnocellular deficit in developmental dyslexics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 209-211.
  499. Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: The magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends in Neurosciences, 20(4), 147-152.
  500. Stemberger, J. P. (1990). Wordshape errors in language production. Cognition, 35, 123-157.
  501. Stemberger, J. P. (1989). Speech errors in early child language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 164-188.
  502. Stéphanovitch, C. (1997). Manuel d’histoire de la langue anglaise des origines à nos jours. Paris: Edition marketing.
  503. Stevens, K. N.(2002).Toward a model for lexical access based on acoustic landmarks and distinctive features. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 111(4), 1872-1891.
  504. Stevens, K. N. (1996). Critique: Articulatory-acoustic relations and their role in speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99, 1693–1694.
  505. Stoodley, C. J., Talcott, J. B., Carter, E. L., Witton, C., & Stein, J. F. (2000). Selective deficits of vibrotactile sensitivity in dyslexic readers. Neuroscience Letters, 295, 13-16.
  506. Storkel, H. L. (2002). Restructuring similarity neighborhoods in the developing mental lexicon. Journal of Child Language Development, 29, 251-274.
  507. Stuart, G. W., McAnally, K. I., & Castles, A. (2001). Can contrast sensitivity functions in dyslexia be explained by inattention rather than a magnocellular deficit? Vision Research, 41, 3205-3211.
  508. Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Mody, M. (1995). Auditory temporal perception deficits in the reading impaired: A critical review of the evidence. Psychonomic Bulletin, 2, 508-514.
  509. Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Schankweiler, D. (1970). Hemispheric specialization for speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 48, 579-594.
  510. Sundara, M., Polka, L., & Genesee, F. (2006). Language experience facilitates discrimination of /d – D/ in monolingual and bilingual acquisition of English. Cognition, 100(2), 186-199.
  511. Swingley, D. (2005). 11-month-olds’ knowledge of how familiar words sound. Developmental Science, 8, 432-443.
  512. Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13(5), 480-484.
  513. Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition, 76, 147-166.
  514. Swingley, D., Pinto, J. P., & Fernald, A. (1999). Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition, 71, 73-108.
  515. Talcott, J. B., Hansen, P. C., Assoku, E. L., & Stein, J. F. (2000). Visual motion perception in dyslexia: Evidence for temporal and energy integration deficits. Neuropsychologia, 38, 935-943.
  516. Talcott, J. B., Hansen, P. C., Willis-Owen, C., McKinnell, C., Richardson, A., & Stein, J. (1998). Visual magnocellular impairment in adult developmental dyslexics. Neuro-Ophtalmology, 20, 187-201.
  517. Tallal, P. (2004). Improving language and literacy is a matter of time. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 721-728.
  518. Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9, 182-198.
  519. Tallal, P., Miller, S., & Fitch, R. H. (1993). Neurobiological basis of speech: A case for the preeminence of temporal processing. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 682, 27-47.
  520. Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1975). Developmental aphasia: The perception of brief vowels and extended stop consonants. Neuropsychologia, 13, 69-74.
  521. Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia: Rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia, 12, 83–93.
  522. Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973). Developmental aphasia: Impaired rate of nonverbal processing as a function of sensory modality. Neuropsychologia, 11, 389-398.
  523. Tallal, P., Stark, R., & Mellits, D. (1985). Identification of language-impaired children on the basis of rapid perception and production skills. Brain and Langage, 25, 314-322.
  524. Tan, L. H., & Perfetti, C. A. (1999). Phonological and associative inhibition in the early stages of English word identification: Evidence from backward masking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 59-69.
  525. Temple, E., Deutsch, G. K., Poldrack, R. A., Miller, S. L., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100, 2860-2865.
  526. Temple, C. M., & Marshall, J. C. (1983). A case study of developmental phonological dyslexia. British Journal of Psychology, 74, 517-533.
  527. Temple, E., Poldrack, R. A., Salidis, J., Deutsch, G. K., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: An fMRI study. NeuroReport, 12(2), 299-307.
  528. Thomson, J. B., Chenault, B., Abbott, R. D., Raskind, W. H., Richards, T., Aylward, E., BErninger, V. W. (2005). Converging evidence for attentional influences on the orthographic word form in child dyslexics. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18, 93-126.
  529. Thoonen, G., Maassen, B., Gabreels, F., & Schreuder, R. (1994). Feature analysis of singleton consonant errors in developmental verbal dyspraxia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 1424-1440.
  530. Treisman, M. (1978). Space or lexicon? The word frequency effect and error response frequency effect. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 37-59.
  531. Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell: A study of first-grade children. New-York: Oxford University Press.
  532. Treiman, R. (1985). Onsets and rimes as units of spoken syllables: Evidence from children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 161-181.
  533. Treiman, R., Broderick, V., Tincoff, R., & Rodriguez, K. (1998). Children’s phonological awareness: Confusions between phonemes that differ only in voicing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 3-21.
  534. Treiman, R., & Breaux, A. M. (1982). Common phoneme and overall similarity relations among spoken syllables: Their use by children and adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 11, 569-598.
  535. Treiman, R., Goswami, U., & Bruck, M. (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: Implications for reading development and theory. Memory and Cognition, 18, 559-567.
  536. Tremblay, K., Kraus, N., Carrell, T. D., & McGee, T. (1997). Central auditory system plasticity: Generalization to novel stimuli following listening training. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(6), 3762-3773.
  537. Tversky, A. (1977). Features of similarity. Phonological Review, 84, 327-352.
  538. Utman, J. A., Blumstein, S. E., & Sullivan, K. (2001). Mapping from sound to meaning: Reduced lexical activation in Broca’s aphasics. Brain and Language, 79, 444-472.
  539. Valdois, S. (2004a). The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia, 10(4), 339-363.
  540. Valdois, S. (2004b). Les sous-types de dyslexies développementales. In S. Valdois, P. Colé, & D. David (Eds.), Apprentissage de la lecture et dyslexie (pp. 171-196). Marseille: Solal.
  541. Valdois, S. (1996). A Case Study of Developmental Surface Dyslexia and Dysgraphia. Brain & Cognition, 32, 229-231.
  542. Valdois, S., Bosse, M. L., Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., Zorman, M., David, D., & Pellat, J. (2003). Phonological and visual processing deficits can dissociate in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from two case studies. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 541-572.
  543. Valdois, S., Bosse, M. L., & Tainturier, M. J. (2004b). The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: Review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia, 10, 1-25.
  544. Valdois, S., Colé, P., & David, D. (2004a). Apprentissage de la lecture et dyslexie développementale: de la théorie à la pratique. Marseille : Solal.
  545. Valdois, S., Gérard, C., Vanault, P., & Dugas, M. (1995). Peripheral Developmental Dyslexia: A Visual Attentional Account? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12(1), 31-67.
  546. Valdois, S., & Launay, L. (1999). Evaluation et Rééducation Cognitives des Dyslexies Développementales: illustration à partir d’une étude de cas. In P. Azouvi, D. Perrier, & M. Van der Linden (Eds.), La rééducation en neuropsychologie: étude de cas (pp. 95-115). Marseille: Solal.
  547. Van den Broecke, M. P. R., & Goldstein, L. (1980). Consonant features in speech errors. In V. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen and hand (pp. 47-65). New York: Academic Press.
  548. Van der Hulst, H. (2005). Molecular structure of phonological segments. In P. Carr, J. Durand, & C. J. Ewen (Eds.), Headhood, elements, specification and contrastivity (pp. 193-234). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  549. Van Ijzendoorn, M.H., & Bus, A.G. (1994). Meta-analytic confirmation of the non-word reading deficit in developmental dyslexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 29, 266-275.
  550. Vanni, S., Uusitalo, M. A., Kiesila, P., & Hari, R. (1997). Visual motion activates V5 in dyslexics. Neuroreport, 8, 1939-1942.
  551. Van Orden, G. C. (1991). Phonologic mediation is fundamental to reading. In D. Besner, & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word Recognition. Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  552. Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound and reading. Memory and Cognition, 15, 181-198.
  553. Van Orden, G. C., & Goldinger, S. D. (1994). Interdependence of form and function in cognitive systems explains perception of printed words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1269-1291.
  554. Van Orden, G. C., Jansen op de Haar, M. A., & Bosman, A. M. T. (1997). Complex dynamic systems also predict dissociations, but they do not reduce to autonomous components. In A. Caramazza (Ed.), Access of phonological and orthographic lexical forms: Evidence from dissociations in reading and spelling (pp. 131-165). UK: Psychology Press.
  555. Van Orden, G. C., Johnston, J. C., & Hale, B. L. (1988). Word identification in reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3), 371-386.
  556. Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What we have learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2-40.
  557. Ventura, P., Kolinsky, R., Querido, J. L., Fernandes, S., & Morais, J. (2007). Is phonological encoding in naming influenced by literacy ? Journal of Psycholinguist Research, 36, 341-360.
  558. Verstaen, V., Humphreys, G. W., Olson, A., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1995). Are phonemic effects in backward masking evidence for automatic prelexical phonemic activation in visual word recognition? Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 335-356.
  559. Viret, A. C. (2003). Décours temporel des effets d’amorçage et de masquage phonétique inter-lettre dans un stimulus écrit: étude auprès d’adultes et d’enfants normo-lecteurs. Mémoire de Maîtrise de Psychologie option Neurosciences, Université Lumière Lyon 2.
  560. Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1999). Probabilistic phonotactics and neighborhood activation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 374-408.
  561. Vitz, P. C., & Winkler, B. S. (1973). Predicting the judged « similarity of sound » of English words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 373-388.
  562. Vogel Sosa, A., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2006).Patterns of intra-word phonological variability during the second year of life. Journal of Child Language, 33(1), 31-50.
  563. Walley, A. C. (1988). Spoken word recognition by young children and adults. Cognitive Development, 3, 137-165.
  564. Wang, M. D., & Bilger, R. C. (1973). Consonant confusions in noise: A study of perceptual features. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54, 1248-1266.
  565. Wang, Y., Spence, M. M., Jogman, A., & Sereno, J. (1999). Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 3649-3658.
  566. Warner, N., Smits, R., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A. (2005). Phonological and statistical effects on timing of speech perception: Insights from a database of Dutch diphone perception. Speech Communication, 46, 53-72.
  567. Waters, G. S., Bruck, M., & Seidenberg, M. (1985). Do children use similar processus to read and spell words? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 511-530.
  568. Wehner, D. T., Ahlfors, S. P., & Mody, M. (2007). Effects of phonological contrast on auditory word discrimination in children with and without reading disability: A magnetoencephalography (MEG) study. Neuropsychologia, 45(14), 3251-3262.
  569. Werker, J. F., & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental model of speech processing. Language Learning and Development, 1, 197-234.
  570. Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K. M., & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants’ ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy, 3, 1-30.
  571. Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49-63.
  572. Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1987). Speech perception in severely disabled and average reading children. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 1, 48-61.
  573. White, S., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Hansen, P., & Swettenham, J., Frith, U., & Ramus, F. (2006). The role of sensorimotor impairments in dyslexia: A multiple case study of dyslexic children. Developmental Science, 9(3), 237-255.
  574. White, K. S., & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Sub-segmental detail in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 114-132.
  575. Whitney, C., & Cornelissen, P. (2005). Letter-position encoding and dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 28(3), 274-301.
  576. Wickelgren, W. A. (1969). Auditory and articulatory coding in verbal short-terme memory. Psychological Review, 76, 232-235.
  577. Wickelgren, W. A. (1966). Distinctive features and errors in short-term memory for English consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 39(2), 388-398.
  578. Wickelgren, W. A. (1965). Acoustic similarity and retroactive inference in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 53-61.
  579. Willcutt, E., & Pennington, B. (2000). Comorbidity of reading disability and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Differences by gender and subtypes. Journal of Learning disabilities, 33, 179-191.
  580. Williams, F. (1979). The modification of speech perception and production in second-language learning. Perception and Psychophysics, 26, 95-104.
  581. Williams, F. (1970). Analysis of production errors in the phonetic performance of school-age standard-English-speaking children. Final Report. Texas University, Austin. Center for Communication Research.
  582. Wilmer, J. B., Richardson, A. J., Chen, Y., & Stein, J. (2004). Two visual motion processing deficits in developmental dyslexia associated with different reading skills deficits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 528-540.
  583. Wimmer, H. (1996). The early manifestation of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from German children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 171-188.
  584. Wimmer, H. (1995). The nonword deficit in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from German children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 80-90.
  585. Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1-33.
  586. Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (1998). Poor reading: A deficit in skill-automatization or a phonological deficit? Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 321-340.
  587. Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Raberger, T. (1999). Reading and dual-task balancing: Evidence against the automatization deficit explanation of developmental dyslexia. Special Series: Prevention and Treatment of Dyslexia. Journal-of-learning-disabilities, 32, 473-478.
  588. Winer, B. J., Brown, D. R., & Michels, K. M. (1991). Statistical principles in experimental design(third edition). New-York: McGraw-Hill.
  589. Witton, C., Talcott, J. B., Hansen, P. C., Richardson, A. J., Griffiths, T. D., Rees, A., Stein, J. F., & Green, G. G. R. (1998). Sensitivity to dynamic auditory and visual stimuli predicts nonword reading ability in both dyslexic and normal readers. Current Biology, 8, 791-797.
  590. Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 123-141.
  591. Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The question of naming speed deficits in developmental reading disabilities: An introduction to the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 19, 1-24.
  592. Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387-407.
  593. Wolf, M., Goldberg O’Rourke, A., Gidney, C., Lovett, M., Cirino, P., & Morris, R. (2002). The second deficit: An investigation of the independence of phonological and namingspeed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 43-72.
  594. Wydell, T. N., Patterson, K. E., & Humphreys, G. W. (1993). Phonologically mediated access to meaning for Kanji: Is a Rows still a Rose in Japanese Kanji ? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 491-514.
  595. Yaniv, I., Meyer, D. E., Gordon, P. C., Huff, C. A., & Sevald, C. A. (1990). Vowel similarity, connectionist models, and syllable structure in motor programming of speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 1-26.
  596. Yap, R., & Van Der Leij, A. (1994). Testing the automatization deficit hypothesis of dyslexia via a dual-task paradigm. Journal of Learning Disability, 27, 660-665.
  597. Yeni-Khomshian, G. H., & Lafontaine, L. (1983). Discrimination and identification of voicing and place contrasts in aphasic patients. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 37, 107-131.
  598. Yeni-Khomshian, G. H., Ludlow, C. L., Rosenberg, J., Fair, C., & Salazar, A. (1986). Lesion locations associated with speech perception deficits following penetrating head injury. Neuropsychologia, 24(5), 631-647.
  599. Yip, M. C. W. (2004). What is similar in phonological-similarity effect? Paper presented at the 26 th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Chicago: Etats-Unis.
  600. Zabell, C., & Everatt, J. (2002). Surface and phonological subtypes of adult developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 8, 160-177.
  601. Zamuner, T. S. (2009). The structure and nature of phonological neighbourhoods in children’s early lexicons. Journal of Child Language, 36(1), 3-21.
  602. Ziegler, J. C., & Jacobs, A. M. (1995). Phonological information provides early sources of constraint in the processing of letter strings. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 567-593.
  603. Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schule-Körne, D. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or Universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169-193.
  604. Ziegler, J. C., Van Orden G. C., & Jacobs, A. M., (1997). Phonology can help or hurt the perception of print. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(3),845-860.