Bibliographie

  1. Abrahamson, A., (1975), "Experimental analysis of the semantics of movements", In Norman, D., Rumelhart, D. (eds.), Explorations in cognition, Freeman.
  2. Airenti, G., (2010), "Ontologie de l'anthropomorphisme", Cycle de conférences sur le développement cognitif de l'intersubjectivité, Maison Suger, Paris, 19 février 2010.
  3. Allen, S., Ozyurek, A., Kita, S., Brown, A., Furman, R., Ishizuka, T. & Fujii, M., (2007), "Language-specific and universal influences in children’s syntactic packaging of Manner and Path: A comparison of English, Japanese, and Turkish", Cognition, 102:16-48.
  4. Ammon, M. & Slobin, D., (1979), "A cross-linguistic study of the processing of causative sentences.", Cognition, 7:3-17
  5. Andren, M., (2008), "Gesture and object manipulation ", Companion to Cognitive Semiotics, #1: Meaning and Materiality.
  6. Antinucci, F. & Miller, R., (1976), "How children talk about what happened", Journal of Child Language, 3:169-189.
  7. Aske, J., (1989), "Path predicates in English and Spanish: a closer look", Proceedings of the annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 15.
  8. Auroux, S., (1994), La révolution technologique de la grammatisation, Mardaga.
  9. Barrett, M., (1986), "Early semantic representations and early word-usage", In Kuczaj, S., Barrett, M. (eds.), The development of word meaning, New York, Springer-Verlag.
  10. Bassano, D., Eme, E. & Maillochon, I., (1998), "Developmental changes and variability in early lexicon: a study of French children’s naturalistic productions", Journal of Child Language, 25.3:493-531.
  11. Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznik, J., Reilly, J. & Hartung, J., (1994), "Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary,", Journal of Child Language, 21.1:85-123.
  12. Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., (1989), "Functionalism and the Competition Model", In MacWhinney, B., Bates, E. (eds.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Sentence Processing, pp. 3-73, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  13. Bates, E., Bretherton, I. & Snyder, L., (1988), From First Words to Grammar : individual differences and dissociable mechanisms, New York, Cambridge University Press.
  14. Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., (1979), "A functionalist approach to the acquisition of grammar", In Ochs, E., Schieffelin, B. (eds.), Developmental pragmatics, pp. 167-211., New York, Academic Press.
  15. Benazzo, S., (2009), "The emergence of temporality: from restricted linguistic systems to early language", In Botha, R., De Swart, H. (eds.), Language Evolution: the View from Restricted Linguistic Systems, pp. 21-57, Utrecht, LOT Occasional Series.
  16. Benedict, H., (1979), "Early lexical development: Comprehension and production", Journal of Child Language, 6:183-200.
  17. Benveniste, E., (1974), "L'appareil formel de l'énonciation", In Benveniste, E. (ed.), Problèmes de linguistique générale, Paris, Gallimard.
  18. Benveniste, E., (1966), Problèmes de linguistique générale, Paris, Gallimard.
  19. Berlin, B. & Kay, P., (1969), Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution, Berkeley, University of California Press.
  20. Berman, R., (2008), “Revisiting English Phrasal Verbs: The Case of Advanced Learner Language”, Study Day in Honor of Elite Olshtain, Jerusalem, 26 mars 2008.
  21. Berman, R. & Slobin, D., (1994), Relating events in narrative: a crosslinguistic developmental study, Hillsdale, Erlbaum.
  22. Bernicot, J., Salazar Orvig, A. & Venezziano, E., (2006), "Les reprises : dialogue, formes, fonctions et ontogenèse", La linguistique, 42.2:29-50.
  23. Bernstein, D. & Tiegerman-Farber, E., (1997), Language and communication disorders in children, Boston, M.A., Allyn & Bacon.
  24. Bernstein, B., (1971), Class, codes and control, Theoretical studies towards a sociology of language, Londres, Routledge & Kegan Paul.
  25. Bertenthal, B., (1993), "Infants’ perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic image and knowledge-based constraints", In Granrud, C. (ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy, pp. 175–214, Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.
  26. Berthonneau, A.M., Cadiot, P. & Anscombre, J.C. (Eds.), (1993), Les Prépositions: méthodes d'analyse, Presses Universitaires de Lille.
  27. Bertoncini, J., de Boysson-Bardies, B., (2000), "La perception et la production de la parole avant deux ans", In Kail, M., Fayol, M. (eds.), L'acquisition du langage: Le langage en émergence, pp. 95-136, Paris, P.U.F.
  28. Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P. & Faubert, J., (2003), "Motion perception in autism: A “Complex” Issue", Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15:1-8.
  29. Bickerton, D., (1990), Language & species, Chicago, University of Chicago Press.
  30. Blake, R., Turner, L., Smoski, M., Pozdol, S. & Stone, W., (2003), "Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism", Psychological Science, 14:151-157.
  31. Blake, J. & de Boysson-Bardies, B., (1992), "Patterns in babbling: a cross-linguistic study", Journal of Child Language, 19:51-74.
  32. Bloom, L., (2000), "The Intentionality Model of Word Learning : How to Learn a Word, Any Word", In Golinkoff, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bloom, L., Smith, L., Woodward, A., Akhtar, N., Tomasello, M., Hollich, G. (eds.), Becoming a Word Learner : A Debate on Lexical Acquisition, pp. 19-50, New York, Oxford University Press.
  33. Bloom, L., (1991), Language development from two to three, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  34. Bloom, L., (1973), One word at a time, La Hague, Mouton.
  35. Bloom, L., (1970), Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars , Cambridge, M.I.T Press.
  36. Bobick, A., (1997), "Movement, Activity and Action : the Role of Knowledge in Perception of Motion", Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.
  37. Bolinger, D., (1971), The phrasal verb in English, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.
  38. Borillo, A., (1998), L’espace et son expression en français, Paris/Gap, Ophrys.
  39. Bornstein, M., Cote, L., Maital, S., Painter, K., Park, S., Pascual, L., Pêcheux, M., Ruel, J., Venuti, P. & Vyt, A., (2004), "Cross-Linguistic Analysis of Vocabulary in Young Children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italien, Korean, and American English", Child Development, 4:1115-1139.
  40. Bottineau, D., (2004), "Traductologie, linguistique et cognition : les procédés de traduction comme correction des écarts typologiques entre l’anglais et le français", In Ballard, M., Hewson, L. (eds.), Correct/Incorrect, pp. 109-122, Artois Presse Université.
  41. Boulenger, V., (2006), Le Langage et l’Action : Dynamique des liens fonctionnels unissant verbes d’action et contrôle moteur, thèse de Doctorat publiée en ligne, Université Lumière Lyon II.
  42. Bowerman, M., Choi, S., (2003), "Space under Construction : Language-Specific Spatial Categorization in First Language Acquisition", In Gentner, D., Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Cognition, Bradford Books.
  43. Bowerman, M., Levinson, S., (2001), "Introduction", In Bowerman, M., levinson, S. (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, Cambridge University Press.
  44. Bowerman, M., Choi, S., (2001), "Shaping meanings for language: universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories", In Bowerman, M., Levinson, S. (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, pp. 475-511, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  45. Bowerman, M., (1996), "Learning how to structure space for language - A crosslinguistic perspective", In Bloom, P., Peterson, M., Nadel, L., Garrett, M. (eds.), Language and space, pp. 385-436., Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.
  46. Bowerman, M., (1994), "From universal to language-specific in early grammatical development", Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 346:37-45.
  47. Bowerman, M., (1993), "Typological perspectives on language acquisition: Do crosslinguistic patterns predict development?", In Clark, E. (ed.), Proceedings of the Twenty-fifth Annual Child Language Research Forum, pp. 7-15., Stanford, CA, Center for the Study of Language and Information.
  48. Bowerman, M., (1985), "What shapes children's grammars? ", In Slobin, D. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, pp. 1257-1319, Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum.
  49. Bowerman, M., (1982), "Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development ", In Wanner, E., Gleitman, L. (eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art, pp. 319-346, New York, Academic Press.
  50. Braine, M., (1976), "Children's first word combinations", Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41.
  51. Bresson, F. & Symposium de l'Association de psychologie scientifique de langue française (Eds.), (1977), La genèse de la parole, Paris, PUF.
  52. Brigaudiot, M. & Danon-Boileau, L., (2002), La naissance du langage dans les deux premières années, PUF.
  53. Brown, R., (1973), A first language: The early stages, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.
  54. Brown, R., (1958), "How shall a thing be called? ", Psychological Review, 65:14-21.
  55. Brugman, C., (1983), The Story of Over, Indiana University Press.
  56. Bruner, J., (1983), Child's Talk: Learning to Use Language, New York, Norton.
  57. Bruner, J., (1975), "The ontogenesis of speech acts", Journal of Child Language, 2:1-19.
  58. Bühler, K., (1926), "Les lois générales d'évolution dans le langage de l'enfant", Journal de psychologie, XXIII.6:597-607.
  59. Butterworth, G. & Hicks, L., (1977), "Visual proprioception and postural stability in infancy. A developmental study", Perception, 6:255-262.
  60. Bybee, J. & Hopper, P., (2001), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, Amsterdam ; Philadelphia, PA, John Benjamins Pub. Co., Collection “Typological studies in language” (vol. 45).
  61. Bybee, J. & Hopper, P., (2001), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
  62. Cabrejo-Parra, E., (2004), "Acquisition du langage et activité psychique", Du jasis ... à la parole : acquisition du langage, Actes du colloque du 6 mars 2004 au Centre Alfred Binet , Editions du papyrus.
  63. Cadiot, P., Lebas, F., Visetti, Y.M., (2006), "The semantics of motion verbs: Action, space, and qualia ", In Hickmann, M., Robert, S. (eds.), Space in Languages : Linguistic Systems and Cognitive Categories, pp. 175–206, John Benjamins, Collection “Typological Studies in Language” (vol. 66).
  64. Cadiot, P., (2002), "Schematics and Motifs in the Semantics of Prepositions", In Feigenbaum, S., Kurzon, D. (eds.), Prepositions in their Syntactic, Semantic and Pragmatic Context, pp. 41-57, Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
  65. Cadiot, P. & Visetti, Y.M., (2001), Pour une théorie des formes sémantiques – motifs, profils, thèmes, Paris, PUF.
  66. Calbris, G., (2003), "From cutting an object to a clear cut analysis: Gesture as the representation of a preconceptual schema linking concrete actions to abstract notions, ", Gesture, 3.1:19-46.
  67. Camaioni, L. & Longobardi, E., (2001), "Nouns versus verb emphasis in Italian mother-to-child speech", Journal of Child Language, 28:773-785.
  68. Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E. & Tomasello, M., (2003), "A construction based analysis of child-directed speech", Cognitive Science, 27:843–873.
  69. Cappelle, B. & Declerck, R., (2005), "Spatial and temporal boundedness in English motion events", Journal of Pragmatics, 37:889-917.
  70. Cardini, F., (2008), "Manner of motion saliency: An inquiry into Italian", Cognitive Linguistics, 19.4:533–569.
  71. Browman, C., (1986), "The hunting of the quark: The particle in English", Language and Speech, 29:311-334
  72. Carter, A., (1975), "The transformation of sensory-motor morphemes into words: A case study of the development of 'more' and 'mine'", Journal of Child Language, 2:233-250.
  73. Casasola, M., Bhagwat, J., Ferguson, K., (2006), "Precursors to verb learning: infants' understanding of motion events", In Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. (eds.), Action meets words How children learn verbs, Oxford University Press.
  74. Chalumeau, P., (1994), L'intonation de la phrase dans la rééducation du langage de l'enfant: le cas des enfants dysphasiques, Thèse de doctorat non publiée, Sciences du langage, sémiotique et didactique, Université de Franche-Comté.
  75. Chapouthier, G., (2001), L'homme, ce singe en mosaïque, Odile Jacob.
  76. Chenu, F., Jisa, H., (2006), "Caused motion constructions and semantic generality in early acquisition of French", In Clark, E., Kelly, B. (eds.), Constructions in Acquisition, pp. 233-261, Stanford, CSLI.
  77. Childers, J., Tomasello, M., (2006), "Are nouns easier to learn than verbs? Three experimental studies", In Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. (eds.), Action meets words. How children learn verbs, Oxford University Press.
  78. Choi, S., (2000), "Caregiver input in English and Korean: use of nouns and verbs in book-reading and toy-play contexts", Journal of Child Language, 27:69-96.
  79. Choi, S., (1997), "Language-specific input and early semantic development: Evidence from children learning Korean", In Slobin, D. (ed.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition (Vol. 5: Expanding Contexts), pp. 41-133, Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  80. Choi, S. & Gopnik, A., (1995), "Early acquisition of verbs in Korean: A cross-linguistic study", Journal of Child Language, 22:497-530.
  81. Choi, S. & Bowerman, M., (1991), "Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: the influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns", Cognition, 41:83-121.
  82. Choi, S., (1988), "The semantic development of negation: A cross-linguistic longitudinal study", Journal of Child Language, 15:517-531.
  83. Chomsky, N., (1986), Knowledge of Language, Praeger.
  84. Chomsky, N., (1965), Aspects of the theory of syntax, Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.
  85. Chomsky, N., (1959), "A Review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior.", Language, 35.1:26-58.
  86. Chomsky, N., (1957), Syntactic structures, The Hague, Mouton.
  87. Christophe, A., Dupoux, E., Bertoncini, J. & Mehler, J., (1994), "Do infants perceive word boundaries? An empirical study of the bootstrapping of lexical acquisition", Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95.
  88. Clancy, P., (1985), "The acquisition of Japanese", In Slobin, D. (ed.), The Cross-linguistic Study of Language Acquisition, Hillsdale, N.J., Erlbaum.
  89. Clark, E., (2005), "Resultant states in early language acquisition ", In Ravid, D., Ben-Zeev Shyldkrot, H. (eds.), Perspectives on language and language and language development, pp. 175-190, Dordrect, Kluwer.
  90. Clark, E., (2003), First language acquisition, Cambridge; New York, Cambridge University Press
  91. Clark, E. & Wong, A., (2002), "Pragmatic Directions about Language Use: Offers of Words and Relations ", Language in Society, 31.2:181-212.
  92. Clark, E. & Wong, A., (2002), "Pragmatic directions about language use: words and word meanings", Language in Society, 31:181-212.
  93. Clark, E., (2001), "Emergent categories in first language acquisition", In Bowerman, M., Levinson, S. (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, Cambridge University Press.
  94. Clark, E. & Carpenter, K., (1989), "The Notion of Source in Language Acquisition", Language, 65.1:1-30.
  95. Clark, E., (1978a), "Discovering what words can do", In Farkas, D., Jacobsen, W., Todrys, K. (eds.), Papers from the parasession on the Lexicon (34-57), Chicago Linguistic Society, April 14-15, 1978, Chicago, University of Chicago Press.
  96. Clark, E., (1978b), "From gesture to word: on the natural history of deixis in language acquisition", In Bruner, J., Garton, A. (eds.), Human Growth and Development, Oxford, Clarendon Press.
  97. Clark, E. & Garnica, O., (1974), "Is he coming or going? On the acquisition of deictic verbs", Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 13:559-572.
  98. Clark, H., (1973), "Space, time, semantics, and the child ", In Moore, T. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language, pp. 27-63, New York, Academic Press.
  99. Clark, E., (1973), "What's in a word? On the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language", In Moore, T. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language, pp. 65-110, New York, Academic Press.
  100. Cleeremans, A. & McCleeland, J., (1991), "Learning the structure of event sequences", Journal of Experimental Psychology, 120:235-253.
  101. Collectif (1997), Cambridge International Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  102. Coupé, C., Marsico, E., Pellegrino, F., (2010), "Les systèmes de sons des langues du monde à la lumière des systèmes dynamiques complexes", In Weisbuch, G., Zwirn, H. (eds.), Qu'appelle-t-on aujourd'hui les sciences de la complexité ?Vuibert.
  103. Coventry, K. & Garrod, S., (2004), Saying, seeing and acting: The psychological semantics of spatial prepositions, Hove & New York, Psychology Press., Collection Essays in cognitive psychology series.
  104. Creissels, D., (2006), Syntaxe générale : une introduction typologique (vol 2 : la phrase), Paris, Hermès – Lavoisier.
  105. Crystal, D., (1979), "Prosodic development", In Fletcher, P., Garman, M. (eds.), Language Acquisition, pp. 33-48, New York, Cambridge University Press.
  106. Crystal, D., (1973), "Non-segmental phonology in language acquisition: a review of the issues", Lingua, 32:1-45.
  107. Culioli, A., (2002), Variations sur la linguistique, Klincksieck.
  108. Culioli, A., (2000), Pour une linguistique de l'énonciation : Domaine notionnel (Tome III), Paris, Ophrys.
  109. Culioli, A., (1999), Pour une linguistique de l'énonciation : Formalisation et opérations de repérage (Tome II), Paris, Ophrys.
  110. Culioli, A., (1990), Pour une linguistique de l’énonciation : Opérations et représentations (tome I) , Paris, Ophrys.
  111. Cuykens, H., Milder, W. & Mortelmans, T. (Eds.), (2004), Adpositions of movement, John Benjamins.
  112. Danon-Boileau, L. & Morgenstern, A., (2009), "Peut-on parler de prédication dans les premiers énoncés de l'enfant?", Faits de Langue, 31-32.La prédication.
  113. Darwin, C., (1872), The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, Londres, John Murray.
  114. Darwin, C., (1845), Journal of Researches into the Natural history and Geology of the Countries Visited During the Voyage of H.M.S. "Beagle" Round the World, under the Command of Capt. Fitz Roy, R.N., Londres, John Murray.
  115. Davidoff, J., Davies, J. & Roberson, D., (1999), "Colour categories in a Stone-Age tribe", Nature, 398:203-204.
  116. Davis, R., (1961), "The fitness of names to drawings", British Journal of Psychology, 52:259-268.
  117. de Boysson Bardies, B., (1996), Comment la parole vient aux enfants, Paris, Odile Jacob.
  118. de Laguna, G., (1927), Speech, its Function and Development, , Yale University Press.
  119. Demuth, K. & Tremblay, A., (2008), "Prosodically-conditioned variability in children’s production of French determiners", Journal of Child Language, 35:99-127.
  120. Demuth, K., Culbertson, J. & Alter, J., (2006), "Word-minimality, epenthesis, and coda licensing in the acquisition of English", Language & Speech, 49:137-174.
  121. Deschamps, A., (2006), "Forme schématique des prédicats : remarques sur quelques verbes de déplacement", Cycnos, 23.1.
  122. Desclés, J.P., (2002), "Categorization : A Logical Approach of a Cognitive Problem", Journal of Cognitive Science, 3.2:85-137.
  123. Diessel, H. & Tomasello, M., (2005), "Particle placement in early child language. A multifactorial analysis", Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 1:89-112.
  124. Dodane, C. & Martel, K., (2009), "Évolution de l'inventaire de contours de Fo chez deux enfants français de 10 à 12 mois : l'importance du contexte pour décrire le stade pré-linguistique", Enfance, 61.3:305-316.
  125. Dodane, C. & Martel, K., (2008), "La prosodie chez le bébé au stade pré-linguistique : premières formes stables",Journées d’études sur la Parole, Avignon.
  126. dos Santos, C., (2009), "Facteurs influençant le calendrier d’acquisition des occlusives", AcquisiLyon 09, Lyon, France, 3-4 Décembre.
  127. Drodz, K., (1995), "Child English pre-sentential negation as metalinguistic exclamatory sentence negation ", Journal of Child Language, 22.3:583-610.
  128. Evans, V., (2010), "From the spatial to the non-spatial: The ‘state’ lexical concepts of in, on and at", In Evans, V., Chilton, P. (eds.), Language, Cognition & Space, Equinox.
  129. Evans, V., Tyler, A., (2004), "Rethinking English "Prepositions of Movement": The Case of To and Through", In Cuyckens, H., de Mulder, W., Mortelmans, T. (eds.), Adpositions of movement, pp. 247-270, Amsterdam, John Benjamins, Collection Belgian Journal of Linguistics,18.
  130. Farwell, C., (1977), "The primacy of goal in the child's description of motion and location", Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 13:126-133.
  131. Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J., Tethick, S. & Reilly, J., (1993), MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual , San Diego, CA Singular Publishing Group.
  132. Ferguson, C. & Farwell, C., (1975), "Words and sounds in early language acquisition", Language, 51:419-439
  133. Fernald, A. & McRoberts, G., (1993), "Effects of prosody and word position on infants’ lexical comprehension",the Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston.
  134. Fey, M., (1986), Language intervention with young children, Allyn & Bacon.
  135. Fillmore, C., (1997), Lectures on deixis , Stanford, CA, CSLI.
  136. Fillmore, C., (1982), "« Frame Semantics »", In Linguistic Society of Korea, $. (ed.), Linguistics in the Morning Calm, pp. 111-138, Seoul, Han Shin Publishing Company.
  137. Fillmore, C., (1977), "Scenes-and-frame semantics", In Zampolli, A. (ed.), Linguistic Structures Processing, pp. 55-81, Amsterdam, North Holland.
  138. Fleischman, S., (1982), "The past and the future: are they coming or going? ",Proceedings of the Eighth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley University.
  139. Fodor, J., (1983), The modularity of mind, Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.
  140. Fortis, J.M., (2007), "Typologie de l'expression linguistique du déplacement : un examen critique ", Séminaire Formes Symboliques, EHESS, Paris, 13 février 2007.
  141. Franckel, J.J. & Paillard, D., (1998), "Aspects de la théorie d'Antoine Culioli ", Langages, 129:52-63.
  142. Franckel, J.J., Paillard, D., Saunier, E., (1997), "Modes de régulation de la variation sémantique d'une unité lexicale. Le cas du verbe passer", In coll. St Cloud, X. (ed.), La locution : entre lexique, syntaxe et pragmatique, Paris, Klincksieck.
  143. François, F., (1984), "Les oppositions verbo-nominales et leur développement chez l’enfant", Modèles linguistiques, VI.1:53-60.
  144. François, F., (1977), La syntaxe de l'enfant avant 5 ans, Paris, Larousse, Collection « Langue et Langage ».
  145. Frank, I., Poulin-Dubois, D. & Trudeau, N., (1997), Inventaire MacArthur du développement de la communication : Mots et énoncés, Montréal, Concordia University.
  146. Furrow, D., Podrouzek, W. & Moore, C., (1990), "The acoustical analysis of children's use of prosody in assertive and directive contexts ", First Language, 10(1).28:37-49.
  147. Gallese, V. & Lakoff, G., (2005), "The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge,", Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22:455-479.
  148. Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Rizzolatti, G., (1996), "Action recognition in the premotor cortex", Brain, 119:593-609.
  149. Gambell, T., Yang, C., (2004), "Statistics learning and universal grammar: Modeling word segmentation", In Sakas, W. (ed.), COLING 2004 Psycho-computational models of human language acquisition , pp. 51– 54, Genève, COLING
  150. Gardam, J., (1996), "The Weeping Child", In Lee, H. (ed.), The Secret Self : A Century of Short Stories by Women , Phoenix Giants.
  151. Gelman, S., Opfer, J., (2002), "Development of the animate-inanimate distinction", In Goswami, U. (ed.), Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, pp. 151 - 166, Oxford, Blackwell.
  152. Gentner, D., (2006), "Why Verbs Are Hard to Learn", In Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. (eds.), Action meets word: How children learn verbs, Oxford University Press.
  153. Gentner, D. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.), (2003), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Cognition, Bradford Books.
  154. Gentner, D., (2003), "Why We're So Smart", In Gentner, D., Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Cognition, Bradford Books.
  155. Gentner, D., (1982), "Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning", In Kuczaj, S. (ed.), Language development, pp. 301-334, Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  156. Gentner, D., (1975), "Evidence for the psychological reality of semantic components : the verbs of possession", In Norman, D., Rumelhart, D. (eds.), Explorations in cognition, Freeman.
  157. Gepner, B., Tardif, C., (2006), "Autism, movement, time and thought. E-motion mis-sight and other temporo-spatial processing disorders in autism", In Vanchevsky, M. (ed.), Frontiers in Cognitive Psychology, pp. 71-101, New York, Nova Sciences Publishers.
  158. Gepner, B., Lainé, F. & Tardif, C., (2005), "E-Motion mis-sight and other temporal processing disorders in autism", Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 23:104-121.
  159. Gepner, B. & Mestre, D., (2002), "Rapid visual-motion integration deficit in autism ", Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6.11:455.
  160. Gepner, B., (2001), "« Malvoyance » du mouvement dans l’autisme infantile ? Une nouvelle approche neuropsychopathologique développementale", La Psychiatrie de l’Enfant, 1:77-126.
  161. Gepner, B., Mestre, D., Masson, G. & de Schonen, S., (1995), "Postural effects of motion vision in young autistic children", NeuroReport, 6:1211-1214.
  162. Ghimenton, A., (2008), Acquisition plurilingue chez un jeune enfant de Vénétie : Etude de la fréquence d’usages des langues et des indices pragmatiques lors des interactions familiales, Thèse de doctorat non publiée, Sciences du langage, Université Stendhal Grenoble III.
  163. Givon, T., (1979), On understanding grammar, New York, Academic Press.
  164. Glenberg, A. & Robertson, D., (2000), "Grounding symbols and computing meaning,", Journal of Memory and Language, 43:379-401.
  165. Gogate, L., Bahrick, L. & Watson, J., (2000), "A study of multimodal motherlese: The role of temporal syncrony between verbal labels and gestures", Child Development, 71:878-894.
  166. Goldberg, A., (2006), Constructions at work: the nature of generalization in language, Oxford, Oxford University Press.
  167. Goldberg, A., (2003), "Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language", Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7.5:219–224.
  168. Goldberg, A., (1998), "Patterns of experience in patterns of language", In Tomasello, M. (ed.), The new psychology of language, Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum.
  169. Goldberg, A., (1997), "The relationship between verbs and constructions ", In Verspoor, K., Sweester, E. (eds.), Current issues in linguistic theory, 150: Lexical and syntactical constructions and the construction of meaning, Philadelphia, John Benjamin.
  170. Goldberg, A., (1995), Constructions : A construction grammar approach to argument structure, Chicago, University of Chicago Press, Collection Cognitive theory of language and culture.
  171. Goldfield, B., (2000), "Nouns before verbs in comprehension vs. production:the view from pragmatics", Journal of Child Language, 27:501-520.
  172. Goldfield, B., (1993), "Noun bias in maternal speech to one-year olds", Journal of Child Language, 20:85-99.
  173. Golinkoff, R. & Hirsh-Pasek, K., (2008), "How toddlers learn verbs", Trends in Cognitive Science, 12.10:397–403.
  174. Golinkoff, R., Chung, H., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Liu, J., Bertenthal, B., Brand, R., Maguire, M. & Hennon, J., (2002), "Young children can extend motion verb labels to point-light displays", Developmental Psychology, 38:604-614.
  175. Golinkoff, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., (2000), "Word learning: Icon, Index, or Symbol?", In Golinkoff, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Bloom, L., Smith, A., Woodward, N., Akhtar, M., Tomasello, G. (eds.), Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical Acquisition, pp. 3-18, Oxford University Press.
  176. Gopnik, A., (1981), "Words and plans: early language and the development of intelligent action", Journal of Child Language, 9:303-318.
  177. Grataloup, C., Hoen, M., Veuillet, E., Collet, L., Pellegrino, F. & Meunier, F., (2009), "Speech Processing: An Interactive Process", Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.
  178. Gries, S., (sous presse), "Acquiring particle placement in English: a corpus-based perspective ", In Pilar Guerrero, M. (ed.), Morphosyntactic alternations in English: functional and cognitive perspectives, London & Oakville, CT, Equinox.
  179. Gries, S., (2008), "Dispersions and adjusted frequencies in corpora", International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 13.4:403-437.
  180. Gries, S., (2003), Multifactorial Analysis in Corpus Linguistics: A Study of Particle Placement, New York/London, Continuum International Publishing Group.
  181. Groussier, M.L., (1997), "Prépositions et primarité du spatial : de l'expression de relations dans l'espace à l'expression de relations non-spatiale", Faits de Langue, 9.
  182. Guillemin-Flescher, J., (1981), Syntaxe comparée du français et de l'anglais : problèmes de traduction, Ophrys.
  183. Gullberg, M., Hendricks, H. & Hickmann, M., (2008), "Learning to talk and gesture about motion in French ", First Language, 28.2:200-236.
  184. Gumperz, J. & Levinson, S. (Eds.), (1996), Rethinking linguistic relativity, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  185. Haeckel, E., (1874), Histoire de la création des êtres organisés d’après les lois naturelles, Paris, Reinwald.
  186. Halliday, M., (1978), Language as Social Semiotic : the social interpretation of language and meaning, Londres, Edward Arnold.
  187. Halliday, M., (1975), Learning how to mean, Londres, Edward Arnold.
  188. Halliday, M., (1973), Explorations in the functions of language , Londres, Edward Arnold.
  189. Hauser, M., Newport, E. & Aslin, R., (2001), "Segmentation of the speech stream in a nonhuman primate: Statistical learning in cotton top tamarins,", Cognition, 78.
  190. Hayward, W. & Tarr, M., (1995), "Spatial language and spatial representation", Cognition 55:39-84.
  191. Hebb, D., (1949), The organization of behavior, New York, John Wiley & Sons.
  192. Hendriks, H., (2005), "Structuring space in discourse: a comparison of Chinese, French, German and English L1 and French, German and English L2 acquisition", In Hendriks, H. (ed.), The structure of learner varieties, pp. 111-156, Berlin, Mouton de Gruyter.
  193. Herr-Israel, E., McCune, L., (2008), "Dynamic event words, motion events and the transition to verb meanings.", In Gagarina, N., Gülzow, I. (eds.), The Acquisition of Verbs and their Grammar : the Effect of Particular Languages, pp. 125-149, Springer.
  194. Herskovits, A., (1988), "Spatial expressions and the plasticity of meaning", In Rudzka-Ostyn, B. (ed.), Topics in Cognitive Linguistics, pp. 271–298., Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
  195. Herskovits, A., (1986), Language and spatial cognition: An interdisciplinary study of the prepositions in English, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  196. Herskovits, A., (1982), Spatial prepositions in English : regularities and irregularities in a complex domain, Thèse de doctorat, Linguistique, Université de Stanford.
  197. Hickmann, M., Hendriks, H., Champaud, C., (2008), "Typological Constraints on Motion in French and English Child Language", In Guo, J., Lieven, E., Budwig, N., Ervin-Tripp, S., Keiko Nakamura, K., Seyda Ozcaliskan, S. (eds.), Crosslinguistic Approaches to the Psychology of Language: Research in the Tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin, Psychology Press.
  198. Hickmann, M. & Hendriks, H., (2006), "Static and dynamic location in French and in English", First Language, 26.1:103-135.
  199. Hickmann, M., (2006), "The relativity of motion in first language acquisition", In Hickmann, M., Robert, S. (eds.), Space in languages: Linguistic systems and cognitive categories, Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
  200. Hickmann, M., (2003), Children's discourse: person, space and time across languages, Cambridge ; New York, Cambridge University Press, Collection Cambridge studies in linguistics ; 98
  201. Hilaire, G., Kern, S., Viguié, A., Dudognon, P., Langue, J. & Romieu, J., (2001), "Le développement communicatif des enfants français de 8 à 30 mois", Le pédiatre, XXXVI.182:7-13.
  202. Hill, L., (1968), Prepositions and adverbial particles: an interim classification semantic, structural and graded, London, Oxford University Press.
  203. Hinton, L., Nichols, J. & Ohala, J. (Eds.), (1994), Sound symbolism, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press
  204. Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. (Eds.), (2006a), Action Meets Word How Children Learn Verbs, Oxford University Press
  205. Hoff, E., (2006), "How social contexts support and shape language development", Developmental Review, 26:55-58.
  206. Højholt, M., 2008, Onomatopoeia as communicative strategy in early language acquisition - a case study, Mémoire de Linguistique, Université d'Aarhus.
  207. Hopper, P. & Traugott, E., (2003), Grammaticalization, Cambridge, UK; New York, NY, Cambridge University Press, (Edition 2nd), Collection Cambridge textbooks in linguistics
  208. Huston, N., (1998), L'empreinte de l'ange, Actes Sud.
  209. Huston, N., (1993), Cantique des plaines, Actes Sud.
  210. Huttenlocher, J., Smiley, P. & Charney, R., (1983), "Emergence of action categories in the child: evidence from verb meanings.", Psychological Review, 90.1:72-93.
  211. Huttenlocher, J., (1974), "The origins of language comprehension", In Solso, R. (ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology, pp. 331-368, New York, Halsted.
  212. Hyltenstam, K. & Viberg, Å., (1993), Progression & regression in language : sociocultural, neuropsychological, & linguistic perspectives, Cambridge [England] ; New York, NY, USA, Cambridge University Press.
  213. Imai, M., Kita, S., Nagumo, M. & Okada, H., (2008), "Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning", Cognition, 109:54-65.
  214. Iverson, J. & Goldin-Meadow, S., (2005), "Gesture paves the way for language development", Psychological Science, 16:367-371.
  215. Iverson, J. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.), (1998), The nature and functions of gesture in children's communication , San Francisco, Jossey-Bass, Collection "New Directions for Child Development".
  216. Jackendoff, R., (1996), "The proper treatment of measuring out, telicity, and perhaps even quantification in English", Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 14.2:305-354.
  217. Jackendoff, R., (1990), Semantic Structures, Cambridge, MA & London, MIT Press.
  218. Jackendoff, R., (1983), Semantics and cognition, Cambridge,MA, MIT Press.
  219. Jamet, D., (1999), "L'image du corps dans les langues", L'ALEPH - Philosophies, Arts, Littératures, 2.2:10-19.
  220. Jespersen, O., (1954), A Modern English Grammar, New York, Barnes and Noble.
  221. Jespersen, O., (1922), Language, its nature, development and origin, New-York, Norton.
  222. Jisa, H., (2003), "Comment l'acquisition du langage nous renseigne sur l'homme", Terrain, 40:115-132.
  223. Jisa, H. & Richaud, F., (1994a), "Quelques sources de variation chez les enfants", AILE, 4:7-51.
  224. Johnson, S. & Aslin, R., (1996), "Perception of Object Unity in Young Infants: The Roles of Motion, Depth, and Orientation ", Cognitive Development, 11:161-180.
  225. Johnson, M., (1987), The Body in the Mind : the Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason, Chicago, University of Chicago Press.
  226. Jusczyk, P. & Aslin, R., (1995), "Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech", Cognitive Psychology, 29.1:1-23.
  227. Kantartzis, K., Kita, S. & Imai, M., (sous presse), "Japanese sound symbolism facilitates word learning in English speaking children", Cognitive Science, 35.
  228. Karmiloff-Smith, A., (1982), "Language as a formal problem space", In Deutsch, W. (ed.), Child language: Beyond description, New York, Springer
  229. Kedar, Y., Casasola, M. & Lust, B., (2006), "Getting there faster: 18- and 24-month-old infants’ use of function words to determine reference", Child Development, 77:325-338.
  230. Kellman, P., Spelke, E. & Short, K., (1986), "Infant perception of object unity from translatory motion in depth and vertical translation", Child Development, 57:72-76.
  231. Kellman, P., (1984), "Perception of three-dimensional form by human infants", Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 36:353–358.
  232. Kennedy, A., (1920), The Modern English Verb-Adverb Combination, Palo Alto, Stanford University Press.
  233. Kern, S. & Gayraud, F., (2010), L'inventaire français du développement communicatif, Grenoble, La Cigale.
  234. Kern, S., (2006), "Adaptation française des inventaires du développement communicatif de MacArthur-Bates: versions courtes et versions longues", Séminaire INSERM, Unité 569, Krémlin-Bicêtre, 27 mars.
  235. Khalifa, J.C., (2001), "Linguistique et traduction : Le cas des verbes de déplacement ", Anglophonia, 10:199-213.
  236. Kinder, A. & Assmann, A., (2000), "Learning artificial grammars: no evidence for the acquisition of rules", Memory & Cognition, 28:1321–1332.
  237. Kinney, D. & Kagan, J., (1976), "Infant attention to auditory discrepancy", Child Development, 47:155-164.
  238. Klee, T., Stokes, S., Wong, A. & Fletcher, P., (2004), "Utterance Length and Lexical Diversity in Cantonese-Speaking Children With and Without Specific Language Impairment", Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47:1396–1410.
  239. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R. & Volkmar, F., (2003), "The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: lessons from autism", Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 358:345-360.
  240. Kochan, A., Morgenstern, A., Rossi, C. & Sekali, M., (2007), "Children's early prepositions in English and French: a social-interactional device",LingO , Oxford.
  241. Köhler, W., (1929), Gestalt psychology, New York, Liveright Publishing Corporation.
  242. Konopczynski, G., (2000), "The Necessity of Interdisciplinarity in Language Acquisition Studies ", Perspectives: Journal for Interdisciplinary Work in the Humanities, 1.2.
  243. Konopczynski, G., (1999), "L’acquisition du système prosodique de la langue maternelle et ses implications pour l’apprentissage d’une L2", In Delcloque, P. (ed.), Speech Technology Applications in CALL, pp. 62-70, University of Albertay Dundee, Philippe Delcloque.
  244. Konopczynski, G., Tessier, S., (1994), "Structuration intonative du langage émergent", In Halford, B., Pilch, H. (eds.), Intonation, pp. 157-192, Tübingen, Gunter Narr Verlag.
  245. Konopczynski, G., (1991), Le Langage Émergent (II) : Aspects Vocaux et Mélodiques, Hambourg , Buske Verlag.
  246. Konopczynski, G., (1990), Le Langage émergent : caractéristiques rythmiques. , Hambourg, Buske Verlag.
  247. Konopczynski, G., (1976), "Étude expérimentale de quelques structures prosodiques employées par les enfants français entre 7 et 22 mois", Travaux de l'Institut de Phonétique de Strasbourg, 7:171-205.
  248. Kopecka, A., (2009), "L'expression du déplacement en français : l'interaction des facteurs sémantiques, aspectuels et pragmatiques dans la construction du sens spatial", Langages, 173:54-75.
  249. Kopecka, A., (2006), "The semantic structure of motion verbs in French: Typological perspectives", In Hickmann, M., Robert, S. (eds.), Space in languages: Linguistic systems and cognitive categories, pp. 83-101., Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
  250. Kuhl, P., Andruski, J., Chistovich, I., Chistovich, L., Kozhevnikova, E., Ryskina, V. & et al., (1997), "Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants", Science, 277:684-686.
  251. Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M., (1999), Philosophy in the flesh : the embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought, Basic Books.
  252. Lakoff, G., (1987), Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind, Chicago, University of Chicago Press.
  253. Lakusta, L. & Landau, B., (2006), "Starting at the end: The importance of goals in spatial language", Cognition, 96:1-33.
  254. Lakusta, L., (2005), Source and Goal Asymmetry in Non-Linguistic Motion Event Representations, PhD, Developmental Psychology, Johns Hopkins University.
  255. Landau, B. & Zukowski, A., (2003), "Objects, Motions, and Paths: Spatial Language in Children With Williams Syndrome", Developmental Neropsychology, 23.1&2:105-137.
  256. Landau, B., (2001), "Perceptual units and their mapping with language ", Advances in Psychology, 130:71-118.
  257. Landau, B. & Jackendoff, R., (1993), ""What" and "where" in spatial language and spatial cognition", Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16:217-265.
  258. Langacker, R., (1988), "A View of Linguistic Semantics", In Rudzka-Ostyn, B. (ed.), Topics in Cognitive Linguistics, Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
  259. Langacker, R., (1987), Foundations of cognitive grammar, Stanford, CA, Stanford University Press.
  260. Lannoy, J., (2008), Langage, perception, mouvement: Blanchot et Merleau-Ponty, Grenoble, Editions Jérôme Millon
  261. Lansari, L., (2009), Les périphrases verbales "aller + infinitif" et "be going to", Paris, Ophrys, Collection "Linguistique contrastive et traduction".
  262. Lassègue, J., (2010), Pour une anthropologie sémiotique ; Recherches sur le concept de forme symbolique, Mémoire original d'habilitation, philosophie, Université Paris IV -Sorbonne.
  263. Ledewag, V., Oller, D. & Lynch, M., (1994), "Infant's vocalization patterns across home and laboratory environment", First Language, 14:49-65.
  264. Lee, P., (1996), The Whorf theory complex: a critical reconstruction, Amsterdam ; Philadelphia, J. Benjamins Pub., Collection Amsterdam studies in the theory and history of linguistic science. Series III, Studies in the history of the language sciences; vol. 81
  265. Lemmens, M., (2005a), "Motion and location: toward a cognitive typology", Parcours Linguistique. Domaine anglais, Actes de l'atelier de linguistique de la SAES, Saint Quentin en Yvelines, Publication de l'université de Saint-Etienne.
  266. Lemmens, M., (2005b), De la sémantique lexicale à la typologie sémantique : vers une sémantique basée sur l'activité langagière, habilitation (document de synthèse), Université Lille3.
  267. Leonard, L. & Finneran, D., (2003), "Grammatical morpheme effect on MLU : “the same can be less” revisited ", Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46:878-888.
  268. Leroy, M., Morgenstern, A., Caët, S., (2010), "L’auto-rectification chez l’enfant: internalisation et appropriation du langage ", In Danon-Boileau, L. (ed.), La rectification. Hommage à Marie-Annick Morel, Ophrys.
  269. Leroy, M., Mathiot, E., Morgenstern, A., (2009), "Pointing gestures, vocalizations and gaze : two case studies", In Zlatev, J., Andren, M., Falck, M., Lundmark, C. (eds.), Studies in Language and Cognition, pp. 261-275, Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
  270. Leslie, A., (1984), "Spatiotemporal continuity and the perception of causality in infants", Perception, 13:287-305.
  271. Lestang, C., (1996), "Langage et handicap physique invalidant", Journal de Pédiatrie et de Puériculture, 9.5:285-291.
  272. Levinson, S., (2003), Space in language and cognition : explorations in cognitive diversity, Cambridge ; New York, Cambridge University Press, Collection Language, culture, and cognition ; 5.
  273. Levinson, S., Kita, S., Haun, D. & Rasch, B., (2002), "Returning the tables: language affects spatial reasoning", Cognition, 84:155-188.
  274. Levinson, S., (2001), "Covariation between spatial language and cognition, and its implications for language learning", In Bowerman, M., Levinson, S. (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, Cambridge University Press
  275. Levinson, S., (1997), "From outer to inner space: Linguistic categories and non-linguistic thinking ", In Nuyts, J., Pederson, E. (eds.), Language and conceptualization, pp. 13-45, Cambridge University Press.
  276. Levinson, S., (1996), "Language and space", Annual Review of Anthropology, 25:353-382.
  277. Levinson, S. & Brown, P., (1994), "Immanuel Kant among the Tenejapans: Anthropology as empirical philosophy", Ethos, 22.1:3-41.
  278. Lewis, C. & Gregory, S., (1987), "Parent's talk to their infants: the importance of context. ", First Language, 14:49-65.
  279. Lieven, E., Pine, J. & Baldwin, G., (1997), "Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development", Journal of Child Language, 24:187-219
  280. Lindner, K. & Johnston, J., (1992), "Grammatical morphology in language-impaired children acquiring English or German as their first language: a functional perspective ", Applied Psycholinguistics, 13:115-129.
  281. Lindner, S., (1981), A lexico-semantic analysis of English verb particle constructions with UP and OUT, Thèse de doctorat non publiée, University of California.
  282. Lucy, J., (1996), "The scope of linguistic relativity: An analysis and review of empirical research.", In Gumperz, J., Levinson, S. (eds.), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity. , pp. 37-69, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  283. Lucy, J., (1992), Language diversity and thought. A reformulation of the linguistic relativity hypothesis, Cambridge University Press, Collection Studies in the social and cultural foundations of language.
  284. Mac Whinney, B., (2008), "Enriching CHILDES for morphosyntactic analysis", In Behrens, H. (ed.), Corpora in Language Acquisition Research : History, methods, perspectives, John Benjamins, Collection Trends in Language Acquisition Research,6.
  285. Mac Whinney, B., (1975), "Pragmatic patterns in child syntax", Stanford Papers And Reports on Child Language Development, 10:153-165.
  286. MacArthur, T., (1998), The Concise Oxford Companion to the English Language, Oxford, Oxford University Press.
  287. MacWhinney, B., (2000), The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk , Mahaw, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, (Edition III).
  288. MacWhinney, B., (1999), "The Emergence of Language from Embodiment", In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), The Emergence of Language, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  289. Mandler, J., (2006), "Actions Organize the infant's World", In Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. (eds.), Action meets words: how children mearn verbs, Oxford University Press.
  290. Mandler, J., (1996), "Pre-verbal representation and language", In Bloom, P., Peterson, M., Nadel, L., Garrett, M. (eds.), Language and space, pp. 365–384, Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.
  291. Mandler, J., (1992), "How to build a baby,", Psychological Review, II: Conceptual primitives:587–604.
  292. Marcos, H., (1987), "Communicative functions of pitch range and pitch direction in infants", Journal of Child Language, 14:255-268.
  293. Markman, E., (1992), Constraints on word learning: speculations about their nature, origins and domain specificity.
  294. Markman, E., (1989), Categorization and naming: Problems of induction, Cambridge, MIT Press;
  295. Marsico, E., Coupé, C., Pellegrino, F. & Hombert, J.M., (2002), "Vers une mesure de la complexité des systèmes phonologiques",IIIèmes Journées d'Etudes Linguistiques, "Universaux sonores", Nantes, France.
  296. Masson, C., (2010), "Les dysfonctionnements de la multimodalité dans l’autisme : une étude de cas entre un et trois ans", Actes du colloque AcquisiLyon, Lyon 3-4 décembre 2010.
  297. Mayer, M., (1969), Frog, where are you?, New York, Dial Books for Young Readers.
  298. McArthur, T. (Ed.), (1998), The Concise Oxford Companion to the English Language, Oxford University Press.
  299. McCune, L., Herr-Israël, E., (sous presse), "Pre-language cognition, motion event semantics, and the transition from single words to first sentences", In Kuehn, N., Bittner, D. (eds.), Lexical bootstrapping: On the central role of lexis and semantics in child language development, Mouton de Gruyter, Collection "Cognitive Linguistic Research Series".
  300. McCune, L., (2006), "Dynamic event words: From common cognition to varied linguistic expression", First Language, 26.2.
  301. McCune, L., Veneziano, E. & Herr-Israel, E., (2004), "Analysis of Motion-Event Semantics in the Transition from Single Words to Early Language Combinations : Evidence from English and French", The Second Lisbon Meeting on Language Acquisition , Lisbonne, Portugal, 1-4 Juin 2004.
  302. McCune-Nicolich, L., (1981), "The cognitive bases of relational words in the single word period", Journal of Child Language, 8:15-34.
  303. McKee, G., Malvern, D. & Richards, B., (2000), "Measuring Vocabulary Diversity Using Dedicated Software", Literary and Linguistic Computing, 15.3:323-337.
  304. Mehler, J., Endress, A., Gervain, J., Nespor, M., (2008), "From perception to grammar", In Friederici, A., Thierry, G. (eds.), Early language development, Amsterdam, Philadelphia, John Benjamins Publishing Company, Collection Trends in Language Acquisition Research,5
  305. Meltzoff, A. & Moore, M., (1979), "Interpreting “imitative” responses in early infancy", Science, 205:217-219
  306. Meltzoff, A. & Moore, M., (1977), "Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates", Science, 198:75-78.
  307. Menyuk, P., (1994), "Word acquisition: The important role of prosody", The Study of Sounds (Special Issue of the Phonetics Society of Japan), 23:209-226
  308. Menyuk, P., (1968), "Children's learning and reproduction of grammatical and nongrammatical phonological sequences", Child Development, 39:849-859.
  309. Miller, G. & Johnson-Laird, P., (1976), Language and perception, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.
  310. Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H. & Plaisted, K., (2002), "High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism", Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43:255-263.
  311. Morgenstern, A., Parisse, C. & Sekali, M., (2010), "Les premières prépositions chez l'enfant : grammaticalisation de l'espace relationnel", Faits de Langue, 34:95-108.
  312. Morgenstern, A., (2009), L’enfant dans la langue, Paris, Presses de la Sorbonne Nouvelle
  313. Morgenstern, A. & Parisse, C., (2007), "Codage et interprétation du langage spontané d’enfants de 1 à 3 ans", Corpus, 6:55-78.
  314. Morgenstern, A., (2006), Un JE en construction : genèse de l’auto-désignation chez l’enfant, Ophrys
  315. Morgenstern, A., (1995), L’enfant apprenti-énonciateur, thèse de Doctorat non publiée, linguistique anglaise, Université Paris III
  316. Nadel, J., Potier, C., (2002), "Imiter et être imité dans le développement de l’intentionnalité", In Nadel, J., Decety, J. (eds.), Imiter pour découvrir l’humain : Psychologie, neuropsychologie, robotique et philosophie de l’esprit, pp. 83-104, Paris, PUF.
  317. Nadel, J., (1986), Imitation et communication entre jeunes enfants, Paris, PUF.
  318. Naigles, L. & Hoff-Ginsberg, E., (1998), "Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use", Journal of Child Language, 25:95-120.
  319. Nelson, K., (2007), Young minds in social worlds : Experience, meaning and memory, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.
  320. Nelson, K., (1995), "The Dual Category Problem in the Acquisition of Action Words", In Tomasello, M., Merriman, W. (eds.), Beyond Names for Things: Young Children's Acquisition of Verbs , pp. 223-249, Hillsdale, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  321. Nelson, K., (1985), Making sense : the acquisition of shared meaning, New York, Academic Press.
  322. Nelson, K., (1974), "Concept, word, and sentence: Interrelations in acquisition and development", Psychological Review, 81:267-285.
  323. Nelson, K., (1973), "Structure and strategy in learning to talk", Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 149.38:1-135.
  324. Ninio, A., (1999b), "Pathbreaking verbs in syntactic development and the question of prototypical transitivity", Journal of Child Language, 26:619-753.
  325. Ninio, A., (1999a), "Model learning in syntactic development: intransitive verbs", International Journal of Bilingualism, 3:111-31
  326. Ninio, A., (1979), "The naive theory of the infant and other maternal attitudes in two subgroups in Israel,", Child Development, 50:976-980.
  327. Nørgaard Jørgensen, R., Dale, P., Bleses, D. & Fenson, L., (2009), "CLEX: A cross-linguistic lexical norms database", Journal of Child Language, 37.02:419-428.
  328. Ochs, E., (1986), "Introduction", In Schieffelin, B., Ochs, E. (eds.), Language Socialization across Cultures, pp. 1-13, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  329. Ochs, E., (1979), "Planned and unplanned discourse", In Givon, T. (ed.), Discourse and syntax, pp. 51-80, New York, Academic Press, Collection Syntax and Semantics,12.
  330. Ochs, E., (1979), "Transcription as Theory", In Ochs, E., Schieffelin, B. (eds.), Developmental Pragmatics, Academic Press.
  331. Ochsenbauer, A.K., (2010), The impact of language-specific factors in first language acquisition: The expression of motion in French and German , Thèse de doctorat, Sciences du Langage, Université Paris 8.
  332. Ogura, T., Dale, P., Yamashita, Y., Murase, T. & Mathieu, A., (2006), "The use of nouns and verbs by Japanese children and their caregivers in book-reading and toy-playing contexts", Journal of Child Language, 33.
  333. Olguin, R. & Tomasello, M., (1993), "Twenty-five-month-old children do not have a grammatical category of verb", Cognitive Development, 8:245-272.
  334. Opfer, J., (2002), "Identifying living and sentient kinds from dynamic information: the case of goal-directed versus aimless autonomous movement in conceptual change", Cognition, 86:97-122.
  335. Ozcaliskan, S. & Goldin-Meadow, S., (2005), "Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development", Cognition, 96:101-113.
  336. Parisse, C. & Poulain, C., (2010), "Shared semantic properties of nouns and verbs in French-speaking children before age two  ", VI International Conference on Language Acqusition, Barcelone, septembre 2010.
  337. Parisse, C. & Le Normand, M.T., (2006), "Une méthode pour évaluer la production du langage spontané chez l’enfant de 2 à 4 ans.", Glossa, 97:20-41
  338. Parisse, C., (2003), "Le débat inné-acquis et le développement du langage à l'aube du 21ème siècle", Intellectica, 35:269-285.
  339. Parisse, C. & Le Normand, M.T., (2000), "Automatic disambiguation of morphosyntax in spoken language corpora", Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32.3:468-481.
  340. Parker, M. & Brorson, K., (2005), "A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MULm) and mean length of utterance of words (MULw)", First Language, 3.75:365-376.
  341. Paul, R., (2000), Language disorders from infancy through adolescents, Mosby-year book.
  342. Pea, R., (1980), "The development of negation in early child language", In Olson, R. (ed.), The social foundations of language and thought, New York, Norton.
  343. Pêcheux., M.G., (1999), "Comment les parents se représentent-ils le développement précoce ?", L'année psychologique, 99.4:709-730.
  344. Pederson, E., Danzigier, E., Wilkins, D., Levinson, S., Kita, S. & Senft, G., (1998), "Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization ", Language, 74.3:557-589.
  345. Peters, A., (1977), "Language learning strategies: does the whole equal the sum of the parts?", Language, 53:560-573.
  346. Piaget, J., (1962), Le langage et la pensée chez l'enfant, Neuchâtel, Paris, Delachaux et Niestlé.
  347. Piaget, J., (1946), Les notions de mouvement et de vitesse chez l'enfant, Paris , PUF.
  348. Piaget, J., (1945), La formation du symbole chez l'enfant: imitation, jeu et rêve, image et représentation, Neuchâtel ; Paris, Delachaux et Niestlé.
  349. Pine, J., Lieven, E. & Rowland, C., (1996), "Observational and checklist measures of vocabulary composition: what do they mean?", Journal of Child Language, 23:573-590.
  350. Pine, J. & Lieven, E., (1993), "Reanalysing rote-learned phrases : individual differences in the transition to multi-word speech,", Journal of Child Language, 20:551-571.
  351. Pinker, S., (1995), The Language Instinct, New York, Harper Perennial.
  352. Pinker, S., (1989), Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of verb argument structure, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.
  353. Pinker, S., (1984), Language learnability and language development, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.
  354. Pourcel, S. & Kopecka, A., (2005), "Motion expression in French: Typological diversity", Durham and Newcastle Working Papers in Linguistics, 11:139-153.
  355. Pruden, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Maguire, M., Meyer, M., (2004), "Foundations of verb learning: Infants categorize path and manner in motion events", Proceedings of the 28th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, Cascadilla Press.
  356. Pulverman, R., Sootsman, J., Golinkoff, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., (2003), "The role of lexical knowledge in nonlinguistic event processing: English-speaking infants’ attention to manner and path", Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, Cascadilla Press.
  357. Pulvermüller, F., (2005), "Brain mechanisms linking language and action.", Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6:576-582.
  358. Pulvermüller, F., (1999), "Words in the brain's language. ", Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22:253-279.
  359. Quayle, N., (1994), Up et le verbe à particule en anglais contemporain, Lille, Presses Universitaires de Lille.
  360. Rakison, D., (2003), "Parts, categorization, and the animate-inanimate distinction in infancy", In Rakison, D., Oakes, L. (eds.), Early concept and category development: Making sense of the blooming buzzing confusion, New York, Oxford University Press.
  361. Quine, W., (1960), Word and object, Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.
  362. Richards, B. & Malvern, D., (1997), Type-Token and Type-Type Measures of Vocabulary Diversity and Lexical Style: an Annotated Bibliography, Reading, Faculty of Education and Community Studies, the University of Reading.
  363. Richards, B., (1990), Language Development and Individual Di¤erences: A Study of Auxiliary Verb Learning, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  364. Riesman, P., (1992), First find your child a good mother : the construction of self in two African communities, New Brunswick, New Jersey, Rutgers University Press.
  365. Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M., (1998), "Language within our grasp," Trends in Neurosciences, 21.5:188-194.
  366. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V. & Fogassi, L., (1996), "Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions", Cognitive Brain Research, 3:131-141.
  367. Robb, M. & Saxman, H., (1990), "Syllable duration of preword and early word vocalisations", Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33:583-593
  368. Robert, S. & Chapouthier, G., (2006), "La mosaïque du langage. ", Marges linguistiques, 11
  369. Rogers, S. & Williams, J. (Eds.), (2006), Imitation and the Social Mind. Autism and Typical Development, New York, Guilford Publications
  370. Rohlfing, K. & Wrede, B., (2010), "From action to language and back: Comment on ‘Grounding language in action and perception: From cognitive agents to humanoid robots’ by Cangelosi", Physics of Life Reviews, 7:152-153.
  371. Rohlfing, K., Choi, S. & Casasola, M., (2010), "Maternal spatial language to English-, Spanish-, Korean- and German-learning infants and how it changes in the course of development: a longitudinal study", Child Language Seminar, Londres, 23-25 juin 2010.
  372. Rondal, J., (1983), L'interaction adulte-enfant et la construction du langage, Bruxelles, Pierre Mardaga.
  373. Rosch Heider, E., (1972), "Universals in color naming and memory ", Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93:10-20.
  374. Rossi, C. & Parisse, C., (Soumis), "Making up categories", Journal of French Language Studies, special issue on First Language Acquisition of French Grammar
  375. Rossi, C. & Shimanek, C., (2007), "Contradiciton et émergence du langage", Journée d'étude de l'Ecole doctorale HSH, Université Lyon 2, 1er juin 2007.
  376. Rossi,, C., Morgenstern, A., (2008), "Outils et méthodes de recherche en acquisition du langage : de la complémentarité entre statistiques et analyse linguistique",JADT, Lyon, Presses Universitaires de Lyon.
  377. Rowland, C., (2007), "Explaining errors in children’s questions", Cognition, 104:106–134.
  378. Ruff, H., (1985), "Detection of Information Specifying the Motion of Objects by 3- and 5-Month-Old Infants", Developmental Psychology, 21.2:295-305.
  379. Sablayrolles, P., (1995), Sémantique formelle de l’expression du mouvement, Thèse de doctorat, linguistique, Université Paul Sabatier, Toulouse.
  380. Sacks, H., (1984), "Notes on Methodology", In Atkinson, J., Heritage, J. (eds.), Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  381. Saffran, J., Aslin, R. & Newport, E., (1996), "Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants," Science, 274:1926-1928
  382. Sarda, L., (1996), "Éléments pour une typologie des verbes de déplacement transitifs directs du français", Les Cahiers de Grammaire, 21:93-121.
  383. Sauvage, J., (2003), "Développement langagier, chaos et interactions sociales.", Langage et société, 105.3.
  384. Schieffelin, B., Ochs, E., (1983), "A cultural perspective on the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication", In Golinkoff, R. (ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication, pp. 115-131, Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  385. Schlesinger, I., (1977), "The role of cognitive development and linguistic input in language acquisition", Journal of Child Language, 4:152-169.
  386. Schmidtke-Bode, K., (2009), "Going-to-V and gonna-V in child language: A quantitative approach to constructional development", Cognitive Linguistics, 20.3:509–538.
  387. Sekali, M., Morgenstern, A., (2009), "What can child language tell us about prepositions ? A contrastive corpus-based study of cognitive and social-pragmatic factors", In Zlatev, J., Andrén, M., Johansson Falck, M., Lundmark, C. (eds.), Studies in Language and Cognition, pp. 261-275, Newcastle upon Tyne, Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
  388. Serres, E., (1824), Anatomie comparée du cerveau dans les quatre classes des animaux vertébrés, Paris, Gabon et Cie.
  389. Shady, M. & Gerken, L., (1999), "Grammatical and caregiver cues in early sentence comprehension", Journal of Child Language, 26:1-13.
  390. Shi, R. & Cyr, M., (2008), "Processing functional morphemes and early lexical acquisition", Communication présentée lors du XXIX Congrès International de Psychologie, Berlin, Juillet 2008.
  391. Sinha, C. & Kuteva, T., (1995), "Distributed Spatial Semantics", Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 18:167-199.
  392. Sinha, C., Thorseng, L., Hayashi, M. & Plunkett, K., (1994), "Comparative Spatial Semantics and Language Acquisition: Evidence from Danish, English, and Japanese", Journal of Semantics, 11.4:253-287.
  393. Skinner, B., (1957), Verbal behavior, New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, Inc.
  394. Slater, A., Morison, V., Town, C. & Rose, D., (1985), "Movement perception and identity constancy in the new-born baby ", British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3:211-220.
  395. Slobin, D., Bowerman, M., Brown, P., Eisenbeiß, S., Narasimhan, B., (sous presse), "Putting things in places: Developmental consequences of linguistic typology", In Bohnemeyer, J., Pederson, E. (eds.), Event representation, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  396. Slobin, D. & Bowerman, M., (2007), "Interfaces between linguistic typology and child language research", Linguistic Typology, 11.1:213-226.
  397. Slobin, D., (2004b), "The many ways to search for a frog: Linguistic typology and the expression of motion events ", In Strömqvist, S., Verhoeven, L. (eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives, pp. 219-257, Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  398. Slobin, D., (2004a), "From ontogenesis to phylogeneis: what can child language tell us about Language evolution?" In Langer, J., Parker, S., Milbrath, C. (eds.), Biology and Knowledge revisited: From neurogenesis to psychogenesis, pp. 255-285, Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
  399. Slobin, D., (2003), "Language and thought online: Cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity ", In Gentner, D., Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the investigation of language and thought, pp. 157-191., Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.
  400. Slobin, D., (2000), "Verbalized events. A dynamic approach to linguistic relativity and determinism", In Niemeyer, S., Dirven, R. (eds.), Evidence for Linguistic Relativity, pp. 107-138, Amsterdam, John Benjamins.
  401. Slobin, D., (1997a), "The Universal, the Typological, and the Particular in Acquisition ", In Slobin, D. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of langage acquisition: vol. V. Expanding the Contexts, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  402. Slobin, D., (1997), "The origins of grammaticizable notions: beyond the individual mind", In Slobin, D. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Laurence Erlbaum Associates
  403. Slobin, D., (1996b), "From 'thought and language' to 'thinking for speaking'", In Gumperz, J., Levinson, S. (eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity, pp. 70-96, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press
  404. Slobin, D., (1996a), "Two ways to travel: Verbs of motion in English and Spanish", In Shibatani, M., Thompson, S. (eds.), Essays in semantics, pp. 195-217, Oxford, Oxford University Press
  405. Slobin, D. & Hoiting, N., (1994), "Reference to movement in spoken and signed languages: Typological considerations", Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 20:487-505.
  406. Slobin, D., (1985), "Why study acquisition crosslinguistically? ", In Slobin, D. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquistion : Vol.I. The Data, pp. 3-24. , Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  407. Slobin, D., (1982), "Universal and particular in the acquisition of language", In Gleitman, H., Wanner, E. (eds.), Language acquisition: the state of the art, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press
  408. Slobin, D., (1977), "Language change in childhood and in history", In Macnamara, J. (ed.), Language learning and thought, New York, Academic Press, Collection Perspectives in neurolinguistics and psycholinguistics series
  409. Slobin, D., (1973), "Cognitive prerequisites for the acquisition of grammar", In Ferguson, C., Slobin, D. (eds.), Studies of child language development, pp. 175–208, New York, Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  410. Smiley, P., Huttenlocher, J., (1995), "Conceptual Development and the Child's Early Words for Events, Objects and Persons", In Tomasello, M., Merriman, W. (eds.), Beyond Names for Things: Young Children's Acquisition of Verbs. , pp. 21-61, Hillsdale, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
  411. Snow, D., (2006), "Regression and Reorganization of Intonation Between 6 and 23 Months", Child Development, 77.2:281-296
  412. Snow, D., (1997), "Children's acquisition of speech timing in English: a comparative study of VOT and final syllable vowel lengthening", Journal of Child Language, 24.1:35-56
  413. Song, J., Sundara, M. & Demuth, K., (2009), "Phonological Constraints on Children's Production of English Third Person Singular –s", Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52:623-642.
  414. Spelke, E., (1990), "Cognitive capacities of human infants: Conceptions of object motion", In Edelman, G. (ed.), Signal and sense: Local and global order in the nervous system, New York, Wiley.
  415. Spelke, E., Born, W. & Chu, F., (1983), "Perception of moving, sounding objects by four month old infants", Perception, 12:719-732
  416. Spencer, J., O’Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson, J. & Wattam-Bell, J., (2000), "Motion processing in autism; evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency", NeuroReport, 11:2765-2767.
  417. Spitz, R., (1957), No and yes: On the genesis of human communication, New York, International Universities Press.
  418. Stern, C. & Stern, W., (1907), Die Kindersprache – Eine psychologishe Untersuchung, Leipszig, Barth
  419. Stosic, D., (2009), "La notion de « manière » dans la sémantique de l’espace", Langages, 3.175:103-121.
  420. Talmy, L., (2000b), Toward a Cognitive Semantics: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring, Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.
  421. Talmy, L., (2000a), Toward a Cognitive Semantics: Concept Structuring Systems, Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.
  422. Talmy, L., (1985), "Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms", In Shopen, T. (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, pp. 249-291, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  423. Talmy, L., (1983), "How language structures space", In Pick, H., Acredolo, L. (eds.), Spatial Orientation: Theory, Research, and Application, pp. 225–282, New York, Plenum Press.
  424. Tardif, T., Gelman, A. & Xu, F., (1999), "Putting the Noun Bias in Context: A Comparison of English and Mandarin", Child Development, 70.3:620-635.
  425. Tardif, T., Shatz, M. & Naigles, L., (1997), "Caregiver speech and children's use of nouns versus verbs : a comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin", Journal of Child Language, 24:535-565.
  426. Tardif, T., (1996), "Nouns are not always learned before verbs: Evidence from Mandarin speakers' early vocabularies", Developmental Psychology, 32:492-504
  427. Theakston, A., Lieven, E., Pine, J. & Rowland, C., (2004), "Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax", Journal of Child Language, 31:61-99
  428. Thompson, S. & Newport, E., (2007), "Statistical Learning of Syntax: The Role of Transitional Probability", Language Learning and Development, 3:1-42.
  429. Thordardottir, E., (2005), "Early lexical and syntactic development in Quebec French and English: implications for cross-linguistic and bilingual assessment", International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40.3:243-278.
  430. Tomasello, M. & Stahl, D., (2004), "Sampling children's spontaneous speech: How much is enough?" Journal of Child Language, 31:101-121
  431. Tomasello, M., (2003), Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition , Cambridge (Mass.) ; London , Harvard University Press.
  432. Tomasello, M., (2001), "Perceiving intentions and learning words in the second year of life", In Bowerman, M., Levinson, S. (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  433. Tomasello, M., (2000), "The social-pragmatic theory of word-learning", Pragmatics, 10.4:401-413.
  434. Tomasello, M., Brooks, P., (1999), "Early syntactic development: A construction grammar approach", In Barrett, M. (ed.), The development of language, pp. 161-190, Hove, UK, Psychology Press, Collection Studies in Developmental Psychology
  435. Tomasello, M., (1999), The cultural origins of human cognition, Cambridge, Mass., Harvard University Press
  436. Tomasello, M., (1992), First verbs: A case study of early grammatical development, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press
  437. Tomasello, M. & Kruger, A., (1992), "Joint attention on actions: acquiring verbs in ostensive and non-ostensive contexts.", Journal of Child Language, 19.2:311-333
  438. Tomasello, M., (1987), "Learning to use prepositions: a case study", Journal of Child Language, 14:79-98.
  439. Toro, J. & Trobalon, J., (2005), "Statistical computations over a speech stream in a rodent", Perception & psychophysics, 67.5:867-875
  440. Toupin, F., (2006), "Les sens procéduraux intersubjectifs de la particule UP", CORELA, 4.2
  441. Turkay, F., Kern, S. & Rossi, C., (2008), "Pragmatic and contextual differences in French and Turkish Caregivers’ input", XI International Congress For The Study Of Child Language , Edinburgh, 28 juillet au 1er Août
  442. Vandeloise, C., (2002), "Relativité linguistique et cognition ", Carnets de grammaire, 9.
  443. Vandeloise, C., (1994), "Methodology and analyses of the preposition in", Cognitive Linguistics, 5.2:157–184.
  444. Vandeloise, C., (1992), "Méthodologie et analyses de la préposition dans", Lexique, 11:15-40.
  445. Vandeloise, C., (1987b), "La préposition à et le principe d'anticipation", Langue française, 76.1:77-111.
  446. Vandeloise, C., (1987a), "L'expression du mouvement : présentation liminaire", Langue française, 76.1:3-4.
  447. Vandeloise, C., (1986), L'espace en français, Paris, Le Seuil.
  448. Veneziano, E., (2004), "The emergence of expressive options in early child language : a constructivist account. ", In Ravid, D., Bat-Zeev Shyldkrot, H. (eds.), Perspectives on language and language development: Essays in honor of Ruth A. Berman, pp. 203-218, Dordrecht, Springer
  449. Veneziano, E. & Sinclair, H., (2000), "The changing status of 'filler syllables' on the way to grammatical morphemes", Journal of Child Language, 27:461-500.
  450. Victorri, B., (2004), "Les grammaires cognitives", In Fuchs, C. (ed.), La linguistique cognitive, Ophrys
  451. Victorri, B., (1997b), "La polysémie : un artefact de la linguistique ?", Revue de Sémantique et de Pragmatique, 2:41-62.
  452. Victorri, B., (1997a), "La place du mouvement dans la structure sémantique des langues", 6ème école d'été de l'Association pour la Recherche Cognitive, Bonas, Formation du CNRS.
  453. Victorri, B. & Fuchs, C., (1996), La polysémie, construction dynamique du sens, Paris, Hermès.
  454. Vihman, M., Kay, E., de Boysson-Bardies, B., Durand, C. & Sundberg,, U., (1994), "External Sources of Individual Differences? A Cross-Linguistic Analysis of the Phonetics of Mothers' Speech to 1-Year-Old Children ", Developmental Psychology, 30.5:651-662.
  455. Vinay, J.P. & Darbelnet, J., (1958), Stylistique comparée du français et de l’anglais, Montréal, Beauchemin.
  456. Volterra, V., Caselli, M., Capirci, O., Pizzuto, E., (2004), "Gesture and the emergence and development of language ", In Tomasello, M. (ed.), Beyond Nature-Nurture. Essays in Honor of Elizabeth Bates, pp. 3-40, Londres, Lawrence Erlbaum.
  457. Von Hofsten, C. & Rosander, K. (2007), From Action to Cognition, Elsevier.
  458. Vygotsky, L., (1934), Thought and Language, Cambridge, MA, The M.I.T Press
  459. Watson, J., (1924), Behaviourism, New York, People's Institute Publishing Company
  460. Wellman, H. & Johnson, C., (1979), "Understanding of mental processes: a developmental study of remember and forget", Child Development, 50:79-88.
  461. Werker, J., Cohen, L., Lloyd, V., Casasola, M. & Stager, C., (1998), "Acquisition of Word-Object Associations by 14-Month-Old Infants", Developmental psychology, 34.6:1289-1309.
  462. Whiting, B. & Whiting, J., (1975), Children of six cultures : a psycho-cultural analysis, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press
  463. Whorf, B., (1956), Language, thought, and reality. Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf, Cambridge, MA, MIT Press
  464. Woodward, A., (1998), "Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach", Cognition, 69:1-34.
  465. Zadeh, L., (1965), "Fuzzy sets", Information and Control, 8:338-353.
  466. Zangl, R. & Fernald, A., (2007), "Increasing flexibility in children’s online processing of grammatical and nonce determiners in fluent speech", Language Learning and Development, 3:199–231.
  467. Zheng, M. & Goldin-Meadow, S., (2002), "Thought before language: how deaf and hearing children express motion events across cultures", Cognition, 85:145-175.
  468. Zlatev, J., (2005), "What’s in a schema? Bodily mimesis and the grounding of language ", In Hampe, B. (ed.), From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics, pp. 313-343., Berlin, Mouton de Gruyter.
  469. Zlatev, J., (2003), "Beyond cognitive determination: Interactionism in the acquisition of spatial semantics", In Leather, J., van Dam, J. (eds.), Ecology of Language Acquisition, pp. 83-107., Amsterdam, Kluwer Academic Publishers.